Adreamis a succession ofimages,ideas,emotions,andsensationsthat usually occur involuntarily in themindduring certain stages ofsleep.[1]Humans spend about two hours dreaming per night,[2]and each dream lasts around 5–20 minutes, although the dreamer may perceive the dream as being much longer than this.[3]

A painting depictingDaniel O'Connelldreaming of a confrontation withGeorge IV,shown inside athought bubble

The content and function of dreams have been topics of scientific, philosophical and religious interest throughoutrecorded history.Dream interpretation,practiced by theBabyloniansin the third millennium BCE[4]and even earlier by the ancientSumerians,[5][6]figures prominently in religious texts in several traditions, and has played a lead role in psychotherapy.[7][8]The scientific study of dreams is calledoneirology.[9]Most modern dream study focuses on the neurophysiology of dreams and on proposing and testing hypotheses regarding dream function. It is not known where in the brain dreams originate, if there is a single origin for dreams or if multiple regions of the brain are involved, or what the purpose of dreaming is for the body or mind.

The human dream experience and what to make of it has undergone sizable shifts over the course of history.[10][11]Long ago, according to writings fromMesopotamiaandAncient Egypt,dreams dictated post-dream behaviors to an extent that was sharply reduced in later millennia.[clarification needed]These ancient writings about dreams highlight visitation dreams, where a dream figure, usually a deity or a prominent forebear, commands the dreamer to take specific actions, and which may predict future events.[12][13][14]Framing the dream experience varies across cultures as well as through time.

Dreaming and sleep are intertwined. Dreams occur mainly in therapid-eye movement (REM) stage of sleep—whenbrain activityis high and resembles that of being awake. Because REM sleep is detectable in many species, and because research suggests that all mammals experience REM,[15]linking dreams to REM sleep has led to conjectures that animals dream. However, humans dream during non-REM sleep, also, and not all REM awakenings elicit dream reports.[16]To be studied, a dream must first be reduced to a verbal report, which is an account of the subject's memory of the dream, not the subject's dream experience itself. So, dreaming by non-humans is currently unprovable, as is dreaming by human fetuses and pre-verbal infants.[17]

Subjective experience and content

Usha Dreaming Aniruddha(oleographic print)Raja Ravi Varma(1848–1906)

Preserved writings from early Mediterranean civilizations indicate a relatively abrupt change in subjective dream experience betweenBronze Ageantiquity and the beginnings of theclassical era.[18]

In visitation dreams reported in ancient writings, dreamers were largely passive in their dreams, and visual content served primarily to frame authoritative auditory messaging.[19][10][20]Gudea,the king of the Sumerian city-state ofLagash(reignedc.2144–2124 BCE), rebuilt the temple ofNingirsuas the result of a dream in which he was told to do so.[6]After antiquity, the passive hearing of visitation dreams largely gave way to visualized narratives in which the dreamer becomes a character who actively participates.

From the 1940s to 1985,Calvin S. Hallcollected more than 50,000 dream reports atWestern Reserve University.In 1966, Hall and Robert Van de Castle publishedThe Content Analysis of Dreams,in which they outlined a coding system to study 1,000 dream reports from college students.[21]Results indicated that participants from varying parts of the world demonstrated similarity in their dream content. The only residue of antiquity's authoritative dream figure in the Hall and Van de Castle listing of dream characters is the inclusion of God in the category of prominent persons.[22]Hall's complete dream reports were made publicly available in the mid-1990s by his protégéWilliam Domhoff.More recent studies of dream reports, while providing more detail, continue to cite the Hall study favorably.[23]

A soldier dreams: the trenches of WWI.Jan Styka(1858–1925).

In the Hall study, the most common emotion experienced in dreams wasanxiety.Other emotions includedabandonment,anger,fear,joy,andhappiness.Negative emotionswere much more common than positive ones.[21]The Hall data analysis showed that sexual dreams occur no more than 10% of the time and are more prevalent in young to mid-teens.[21]Another study showed that 8% of both men's and women's dreams have sexual content.[24]In some cases, sexual dreams may result inorgasmsornocturnal emissions.These are colloquially known as "wet dreams".[25]

The visual nature of dreams is generally highly phantasmagoric; that is, different locations and objects continuously blend into each other. The visuals (including locations, people, and objects) are generally reflective of a person's memories and experiences, but conversation can take on highly exaggerated and bizarre forms. Some dreams may even tell elaborate stories wherein the dreamer enters entirely new, complex worlds and awakes with ideas, thoughts and feelings never experienced prior to the dream.

People who are blind from birth do not have visual dreams. Their dream contents are related to other senses, such ashearing,touch,smell,andtaste,whichever are present since birth.[26]

Neurophysiology

Dream study is popular with scientists exploring themind–brain problem.Some "propose to reduce aspects of dream phenomenology to neurobiology."[27]But current science cannot specify dream physiology in detail. Protocols in most nations restrict human brain research to non-invasive procedures. In the United States, invasive brain procedures with a human subject are allowed only when these are deemed necessary in surgical treatment to address medical needs of the same human subject.[28]Non-invasive measures of brain activity likeelectroencephalogram(EEG) voltage averaging orcerebral blood flowcannot identify small but influential neuronal populations.[29]Also,fMRIsignals are too slow to explain how brains compute in real time.[30]

Scientists researching some brain functions can work around current restrictions by examining animal subjects. As stated by theSociety for Neuroscience,"Because no adequate alternatives exist, much of this research must[sic]be done on animal subjects. "[31]However, since animal dreaming can be only inferred, not confirmed, animal studies yield no hard facts to illuminate the neurophysiology of dreams. Examining human subjects with brain lesions can provide clues, but the lesion method cannot discriminate between the effects of destruction and disconnection and cannot target specific neuronal groups in heterogeneous regions like the brain stem.[29]

Generation

The Knight's Dream,1655, byAntonio de Pereda

Denied precision tools and obliged to depend on imaging, much dream research has succumbed to thelaw of the instrument.Studies detect an increase of blood flow in a specific brain region and then credit that region with a role in generating dreams. But pooling study results has led to the newer conclusion that dreaming involves large numbers of regions and pathways, which likely are different for different dream events.[32]

Image creation in the brain involves significant neural activity downstream from eye intake, and it is theorized that "the visual imagery of dreams is produced by activation during sleep of the same structures that generate complex visual imagery in waking perception."[33]

Dreams present a running narrative rather than exclusively visual imagery. Following their work withsplit-brainsubjects,GazzanigaandLeDouxpostulated, without attempting to specify the neural mechanisms, a "left-brain interpreter"that seeks to create a plausible narrative from whatever electro-chemical signals reach the brain's left hemisphere. Sleep research has determined that some brain regions fully active during waking are, during REM sleep, activated only in a partial or fragmentary way.[34]Drawing on this knowledge, textbook author James W. Kalat explains, "[A] dream represents the brain's effort to make sense of sparse and distorted information.... The cortex combines this haphazard input with whatever other activity was already occurring and does its best to synthesize a story that makes sense of the information."[35]NeuroscientistIndre Viskontasis even more blunt, calling often bizarre dream content "just the result of your interpreter trying to create a story out of random neural signaling."[36]

Theories on function

For many humans across multiple eras and cultures, dreams are believed to have functioned as revealers of truths sourced during sleep from gods or other external entities.[37]Ancient Egyptians believed that dreams were the best way to receive divine revelation, and thus they would induce (or "incubate" ) dreams. They went to sanctuaries and slept on special "dream beds" in hope of receiving advice, comfort, or healing from the gods.[14]From a Darwinian perspective dreams would have to fulfill some kind of biological requirement, provide some benefit for natural selection to take place, or at least have no negative impact on fitness. Robert (1886),[38]a physician from Hamburg, was the first who suggested that dreams are a need and that they have the function to erase (a) sensory impressions that were not fully worked up, and (b) ideas that were not fully developed during the day. In dreams, incomplete material is either removed (suppressed) or deepened and included into memory.Freud,whose dream studies focused on interpreting dreams, not explaining how or why humans dream, disputed Robert's hypothesis[39]and proposed that dreams preserve sleep by representing as fulfilled those wishes that otherwise would awaken the dreamer.[40]Freud wrote that dreams "serve the purpose of prolonging sleep instead of waking up.Dreams are theGUARDIANSof sleep and not its disturbers."[41]

Grandmother and Granddaughter Dream(1839 or 1840).Taras Shevchenko

A turning point in theorizing about dream function came in 1953, whenSciencepublished theAserinskyandKleitmanpaper[42]establishingREM sleepas a distinct phase of sleep and linking dreams to REM sleep.[43]Until and even after publication of the Solms 2000 paper that certified the separability of REM sleep and dream phenomena,[16]many studies purporting to uncover the function of dreams have in fact been studying not dreams but measurable REM sleep.

Theories of dream function since the identification of REM sleep include:

Hobson'sandMcCarley's1977activation-synthesis hypothesis,which proposed "a functional role for dreaming sleep in promoting some aspect of the learning process...."[44]In 2010 a Harvard study was published showing experimental evidence that dreams were correlated with improved learning.[45]

Crick'sand Mitchison's 1983 "reverse learning"theory, which states that dreams are like the cleaning-up operations of computers when they are offline, removing (suppressing) parasitic nodes and other" junk "from the mind during sleep.[46][47]

Hartmann's1995 proposal that dreams serve a "quasi-therapeutic" function, enabling the dreamer to process trauma in a safe place.[48]

Revonsuo's2000 threat simulation hypothesis, whose premise is that during much of human evolution, physical and interpersonal threats were serious, giving reproductive advantage to those who survived them. Dreaming aided survival by replicating these threats and providing the dreamer with practice in dealing with them.[49]In 2015, Revonsuo proposed social simulation theory, which describes dreams as a simulation for training social skills and bonds.[50]

Eagleman'sandVaughn's2021 defensive activation theory, which says that, given the brain'sneuroplasticity,dreams evolved as a visual hallucinatory activity during sleep's extended periods of darkness, busying the occipital lobe and thereby protecting it from possible appropriation by other, non-vision, sense operations.[51]

Erik Hoelproposes, based on artificial neural networks, that dreams prevent overfitting to past experiences; that is, they enable the dreamer to learn from novel situations.[52][53]

Religious and other cultural contexts

Dreams figure prominently in major world religions. The dream experience for early humans, according to one interpretation, gave rise to the notion of a human "soul,"[54]a central element in much religious thought.J. W. Dunnewrote:

But there can be no reasonable doubt that the idea of a soul must have first arisen in the mind of primitive man as a result of observation of his dreams. Ignorant as he was, he could have come to no other conclusion but that, in dreams, he left his sleeping body in one universe and went wandering off into another. It is considered that, but for that savage, the idea of such a thing as a 'soul' would never have even occurred to mankind....[55]

Hindu

In theMandukya Upanishad,part of theVedascriptures ofIndian Hinduism,a dream is one of three states that the soul experiences during its lifetime, the other two states being the waking state and the sleep state.[56]The earliestUpanishads,written before 300 BCE, emphasize two meanings of dreams. The first says that dreams are merely expressions of inner desires. The second is the belief of the soul leaving the body and being guided until awakened.

Abrahamic

Jacob's dream of aladder of angels,c. 1690.Michael Willmann

In Judaism, dreams are considered part of the experience of the world that can be interpreted and from which lessons can be garnered. It is discussed in the Talmud, Tractate Berachot 55–60.

The ancientHebrewsconnected their dreams heavily with their religion, though the Hebrews weremonotheisticand believed that dreams were the voice of one God alone. Hebrews also differentiated between good dreams (from God) and bad dreams (from evil spirits). The Hebrews, like many other ancient cultures, incubated dreams in order to receive a divine revelation. For example, the Hebrew prophetSamuelwould "lie down and sleep in the temple atShilohbefore the Ark and receive the word of the Lord ", andJosephinterpreted a Pharaoh's dream of seven lean cows swallowing seven fat cows as meaning the subsequent seven years would be bountiful, followed by seven years of famine. Most of the dreams in theBibleare in theBook of Genesis.[57]

Christiansmostly shared the beliefs of the Hebrews and thought that dreams were of a supernatural character because theOld Testamentincludes frequent stories of dreams with divine inspiration. The most famous of these dream stories wasJacob's dream of a ladderthat stretches from Earth toHeaven.Many Christians preach that God can speak to people through their dreams. The famous glossary, theSomniale Danielis,written in the name ofDaniel,attempted to teach Christian populations to interpret their dreams.

Iain R. Edgarhas researched the role of dreams inIslam.[58]He has argued that dreams play an important role in the history of Islam and the lives of Muslims, since dream interpretation is the only way that Muslims can receive revelations from God since the death of the last prophet,Muhammad.[59]According to Edgar, Islam classifies three types of dreams. Firstly, there is the true dream (al-ru’ya), then the false dream, which may come from the devil (shaytan), and finally, the meaningless everyday dream (hulm). This last dream could be brought forth by the dreamer's ego or base appetite based on what they experienced in the real world. The true dream is often indicated by Islam'shadithtradition.[59]In one narration byAisha,the wife of the Prophet, it is said that the Prophet's dreams would come true like the ocean's waves.[59]Just as in its predecessors, theQuranalso recounts the story of Joseph and his unique ability to interpret dreams.[59]

In both Christianity and Islam dreams feature in conversion stories.[citation needed]According to ancient authors, Constantine the Great started his conversion to Christianity because he had a dream which prophesied that he would win thebattle of the Milvian Bridgeif headopted the Chi-Rho as his battle standard."[60][61]

Buddhist

In Buddhism, ideas about dreams are similar to the classical and folk traditions in South Asia. The same dream is sometimes experienced by multiple people, as in the case of theBuddha-to-be,before he isleaving his home.It is described in theMahāvastuthat several of the Buddha's relatives had premonitory dreams preceding this. Some dreams are also seen to transcend time: the Buddha-to-be has certain dreams that are the same as those ofprevious Buddhas,theLalitavistarastates. In Buddhist literature, dreams often function as a "signpost" motif to mark certain stages in the life of the main character.[62]

Buddhist views about dreams are expressed in thePāli Commentariesand theMilinda Pañhā.[62]

Other

Dreaming of the Tiger Spring( hổ bào mộng tuyền ) Statue at Hupao Spring (Hupaomengquan) in Hangzhou, Zhejiang, China

In Chinese history, people wrote of two vital aspects of the soul of which one is freed from the body during slumber to journey in a dream realm, while the other remained in the body.[63]This belief and dream interpretation had been questioned since early times, such as by the philosopherWang Chong(27–97CE).[63]

The Babylonians and Assyrians divided dreams into "good," which were sent by the gods, and "bad," sent by demons.[64]A surviving collection of dream omens entitledIškar Zaqīqurecords various dream scenarios as well asprognosticationsof what will happen to the person who experiences each dream, apparently based on previous cases.[6][65]Some list different possible outcomes, based on occasions in which people experienced similar dreams with different results.[6]The Greeks shared their beliefs with the Egyptians on how to interpret good and bad dreams, and the idea of incubating dreams.Morpheus,the Greek god of dreams, also sent warnings and prophecies to those who slept at shrines and temples. The earliest Greek beliefs about dreams were that their gods physically visited the dreamers, where they entered through a keyhole, exiting the same way after the divine message was given.

Antiphonwrote the first known Greek book on dreams in the 5th century BCE. In that century, other cultures influenced Greeks to develop the belief that souls left the sleeping body.[66]The father of modern medicine,Hippocrates(460–375BCE), thought dreams could analyze illness and predict diseases. For instance, a dream of a dim star high in the night sky indicated problems in the head region, while low in the night sky indicated bowel issues.[67]Greek philosopherPlato(427-347) wrote that people harbor secret, repressed desires, such as incest, murder, adultery, and conquest, which build up during the day and run rampant during the night in dreams.[68]Plato's student,Aristotle(384–322 BCE), believed dreams were caused by processing incompletephysiologicalactivity during sleep, such as eyes trying to see while the sleeper's eyelids were closed.[69]Marcus Tullius Cicero,for his part, believed that all dreams are produced by thoughts and conversations a dreamer had during the preceding days.[70]Cicero'sSomnium Scipionisdescribed a lengthy dream vision, which in turn was commented on byMacrobiusin hisCommentarii in Somnium Scipionis.

Herodotusin hisThe Histories,writes "The visions that occur to us in dreams are, more often than not, the things we have been concerned about during the day."[71]

The Dreamingis a common term within theanimistcreation narrative ofindigenous Australiansfor a personal, or group,creationand for what may be understood as the "timeless time" of formative creation and perpetual creating.[72]

SomeIndigenous Americantribes andMexicanpopulations believe that dreams are a way of visiting and having contact with theirancestors.[73]SomeNative Americantribes have usedvision questsas a rite of passage, fasting and praying until an anticipated guiding dream was received, to be shared with the rest of the tribe upon their return.[74][75]

Interpretation

Joseph Interprets Pharaoh's Dreamc. 1896–1902.Jacques Joseph Tissot(1836–1902).

Beginning in the late 19th century, Austrian neurologistSigmund Freud,founder ofpsychoanalysis,theorized that dreams reflect the dreamer'sunconscious mindand specifically that dream content is shaped by unconscious wish fulfillment. He argued that important unconscious desires often relate to early childhood memories and experiences.[7]Carl Jungand others expanded on Freud's idea that dream content reflects the dreamer's unconscious desires.

Dream interpretation can be a result of subjective ideas and experiences. One study found that most people believe that "their dreams reveal meaningful hidden truths".[76]The researchers surveyed students in the United States, South Korea, and India, and found that 74% of Indians, 65% of South Koreans and 56% of Americans believed their dream content provided them with meaningful insight into their unconscious beliefs and desires. This Freudian view of dreaming was believed significantly more than theories of dreaming that attribute dream content to memory consolidation, problem-solving, or as a byproduct of unrelated brain activity. The same study found that people attribute more importance to dream content than to similar thought content that occurs while they are awake. Americans were more likely to report that they would intentionally miss their flight if they dreamt of their plane crashing than if they thought of their plane crashing the night before flying (while awake), and that they would be as likely to miss their flight if they dreamt of their plane crashing the night before their flight as if there was an actual plane crash on the route they intended to take. Participants in the study were more likely to perceive dreams to be meaningful when the content of dreams was in accordance with their beliefs and desires while awake. They were more likely to view a positive dream about a friend to be meaningful than a positive dream about someone they disliked, for example, and were more likely to view a negative dream about a person they disliked as meaningful than a negative dream about a person they liked.

According to surveys, it is common for people to feel their dreams are predicting subsequent life events.[77]Psychologists have explained these experiences in terms ofmemory biases,namely a selective memory for accurate predictions and distorted memory so that dreams are retrospectively fitted onto life experiences.[77]The multi-faceted nature of dreams makes it easy to find connections between dream content and real events.[78]The term "veridical dream" has been used to indicate dreams that reveal or contain truths not yet known to the dreamer, whether future events or secrets.[79]

In one experiment, subjects were asked to write down their dreams in a diary. This prevented the selective memory effect, and the dreams no longer seemed accurate about the future.[80]Another experiment gave subjects a fake diary of a student with apparently precognitive dreams. This diary described events from the person's life, as well as some predictive dreams and some non-predictive dreams. When subjects were asked to recall the dreams they had read, they remembered more of the successful predictions than unsuccessful ones.[81]

Images and literature

Graphic artists, writers and filmmakers all have found dreams to offer a rich vein for creative expression. In the West, artists' depictions of dreams in Renaissance and Baroque art often were related to Biblical narrative. Especially preferred by visual artists were theJacob's Ladderdream in Genesis andSt. Joseph's dreamsin theGospel according to Matthew.

Many later graphic artists have depicted dreams, including JapanesewoodblockartistHokusai(1760–1849) and Western European paintersRousseau(1844–1910),Picasso(1881–1973), andDali(1904–1989).

In literature, dream frames were frequently used in medieval allegory to justify the narrative;The Book of the Duchess[82]andThe Vision ConcerningPiers Plowman[83]are two suchdream visions.Even before them, in antiquity, the same device had been used byCiceroandLucian of Samosata.

The cheshire cat,John Tenniel(1820–1914), illustration inAlice's Adventures in Wonderland,1866 edition

Dreams have also featured infantasyandspeculative fictionsince the 19th century. One of the best-known dream worlds isWonderlandfromLewis Carroll'sAlice's Adventures in Wonderland,as well asLooking-Glass Landfrom its sequel,Through the Looking-Glass.Unlike many dream worlds, Carroll's logic is like that of actual dreams, with transitions and flexible causality.

Other fictional dream worlds include theDreamlandsofH. P. Lovecraft'sDream Cycle[84]andThe Neverending Story's[85]world of Fantastica, which includes places like the Desert of Lost Dreams, the Sea of Possibilities and the Swamps of Sadness. Dreamworlds, shared hallucinations and other alternate realities feature in a number of works byPhilip K. Dick,such asThe Three Stigmata of Palmer EldritchandUbik.Similar themes were explored byJorge Luis Borges,for instance inThe Circular Ruins.

Modernpopular cultureoften conceives of dreams, as did Freud, as expressions of the dreamer's deepest fears and desires.[86]In speculative fiction, the line between dreams and reality may be blurred even more in service to the story.[87]Dreams may be psychically invaded or manipulated (Dreamscape,1984; theNightmare on Elm Streetfilms, 1984–2010;Inception,2010) or even come literally true (as inThe Lathe of Heaven,1971).[86]

Lucidity

Lucid dreaming is the conscious perception of one's state while dreaming. In this state the dreamer may often have some degree of control over their own actions within the dream or even the characters and the environment of the dream. Dream control has been reported to improve with practiced deliberate lucid dreaming, but the ability to control aspects of the dream is not necessary for a dream to qualify as "lucid" —a lucid dream is any dream during which the dreamer knows they are dreaming.[88]The occurrence of lucid dreaming has been scientifically verified.[89]

"Oneironaut"is a term sometimes used for those who lucidly dream.

In 1975, psychologist Keith Hearne successfully recorded a communication from a dreamer experiencing a lucid dream. On April 12, 1975, after agreeing to move his eyes left and right upon becoming lucid, the subject and Hearne's co-author on the resulting article, Alan Worsley, successfully carried out this task.[90]Years later, psychophysiologistStephen LaBergeconducted similar work including:

  • Using eye signals to map the subjective sense of time in dreams.
  • Comparing the electrical activity of the brain while singing awake and while dreaming.
  • Studies comparing in-dream sex, arousal, and orgasm.[91]

Communication between two dreamers has also been documented. The processes involved includedEEGmonitoring, ocular signaling, incorporation of reality in the form of red light stimuli and a coordinating website. The website tracked when both dreamers were dreaming and sent the stimulus to one of the dreamers where it was incorporated into the dream. This dreamer, upon becoming lucid, signaled with eye movements; this was detected by the website whereupon the stimulus was sent to the second dreamer, invoking incorporation into that dreamer's dream.[92]

Recollection

Raphael's dream(1821).Johannes Riepenhausenand Franz Riepenhausen.

The recollection of dreams is extremely unreliable, though it is a skill that can be trained. Dreams can usually be recalled if a person is awakened while dreaming.[93]Women tend to have more frequent dream recall than men.[93]Dreams that are difficult to recall may be characterized by relatively littleaffect,and factors such assalience,arousal,and interference play a role in dream recall. Often, a dream may be recalled upon viewing or hearing a random trigger or stimulus. Thesalience hypothesisproposes that dream content that is salient, that is, novel, intense, or unusual, is more easily remembered. There is considerable evidence that vivid, intense, or unusual dream content is more frequently recalled.[94]Adream journalcan be used to assist dream recall, for personal interest orpsychotherapypurposes.

Adults report remembering around two dreams per week, on average.[95][96]Unless a dream is particularly vivid and if one wakes during or immediately after it, the content of the dream is typically not remembered.[97]

In line with the salience hypothesis, there is considerable evidence that people who have more vivid, intense or unusual dreams show better recall. There is evidence that continuity of consciousness is related to recall. Specifically, people who have vivid and unusual experiences during the day tend to have more memorable dream content and hence better dream recall. People who score high on measures of personality traits associated with creativity, imagination, and fantasy, such asopenness to experience,daydreaming,fantasy proneness,absorption,andhypnotic susceptibility,tend to show more frequent dream recall.[94]There is also evidence for continuity between the bizarre aspects of dreaming and waking experience. That is, people who report more bizarre experiences during the day, such as people high inschizotypy(psychosis proneness), have more frequent dream recall and also report more frequentnightmares.[94]

Dream-recording machine

Recording or reconstructing dreams may one day assist with dream recall.[98][99]Using the permitted non-invasive technologies, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) andelectromyography(EMG), researchers have been able to identify basic dream imagery,[100]dream speechactivity[101]and dream motor behavior (such as walking and hand movements).[102][103]

Miscellany

Illusion of reality

Some philosophers have proposed that what we think of as the "real world" could be or is an illusion (an idea known as theskeptical hypothesisaboutontology). The first recorded mention of the idea was in the 4th century BCE byZhuangzi,and in Eastern philosophy, the problem has been named the "Zhuangzi Paradox."

He who dreams of drinking wine may weep when morning comes; he who dreams of weeping may in the morning go off to hunt. While he is dreaming he does not know it is a dream, and in his dream he may even try to interpret a dream. Only after he wakes does he know it was a dream. And someday there will be a great awakening when we know that this is all a great dream. Yet the stupid believe they are awake, busily and brightly assuming they understand things, calling this man ruler, that one herdsman—how dense! Confucius and you are both dreaming! And when I say you are dreaming, I am dreaming, too. Words like these will be labeled the Supreme Swindle. Yet, after ten thousand generations, a great sage may appear who will know their meaning, and it will still be as though he appeared with astonishing speed.[104]

The idea also is discussed in Hindu and Buddhist writings.[105]It was formally introduced to Western philosophy byDescartesin the 17th century in hisMeditations on First Philosophy.

Absent-minded transgression

Dreams of absent-minded transgression (DAMT) are dreams wherein the dreamer absent-mindedly performs an action that he or she has been trying to stop (one classic example is of a quitting smoker having dreams of lighting a cigarette). Subjects who have had DAMT have reported waking with intense feelings ofguilt.One study found a positive association between having these dreams and successfully stopping the behavior.[106]

Non-REM dreams

Hypnogogic and hypnopompic dreams, dreamlike states shortly after falling asleep and shortly before awakening, and dreams during stage 2 ofNREM-sleep,also occur, but are shorter than REM-dreams.[107][108]

Daydreams

Dante Meditating,1852, byJoseph Noel Paton

A daydream is a visionaryfantasy,especially one of happy, pleasant thoughts, hopes or ambitions, imagined as coming to pass, and experienced while awake.[109]There are many different types of daydreams, and there is no consistent definition amongst psychologists.[109]The general public also uses the term for a broad variety of experiences. Research by Harvard psychologistDeirdre Barretthas found that people who experience vivid dreamlikemental imagesreserve the word for these, whereas many other people refer to milder imagery, realistic future planning, review of memories or just "spacing out" —i.e. one's mind going relatively blank—when they talk about "daydreaming".[110][111]

While daydreaming has long been derided as a lazy, non-productive pastime, it is now commonly acknowledged that daydreaming can be constructive in some contexts.[112]There are numerous examples of people in creative or artistic careers, such as composers, novelists and filmmakers, developing new ideas through daydreaming. Similarly, research scientists, mathematicians and physicists have developed new ideas by daydreaming about their subject areas.

Hallucination

A hallucination, in the broadest sense of the word, is aperceptionin the absence of astimulus.In a stricter sense, hallucinations are perceptions in a conscious and awake state, in the absence of external stimuli, and have qualities of real perception, in that they are vivid, substantial, and located in external objective space. The latter definition distinguishes hallucinations from the related phenomena of dreaming, which does not involve wakefulness.

Nightmare

Woman having a nightmare. Jean-Pierre Simon (1764–1810 or 1813).

A nightmare is an unpleasant dream that can cause a strongnegative emotionalresponse from the mind, typicallyfearorhorror,but alsodespair,anxietyand greatsadness.The dream may contain situations of danger, discomfort, psychological or physical terror. Sufferers usually awaken in a state of distress and may be unable to return tosleepfor a prolonged period of time.[113]

Night terror

A night terror, also known as a sleep terror orpavor nocturnus,is aparasomniadisorderthat predominantly affects children, causing feelings of terror or dread. Night terrors should not be confused withnightmares,which are bad dreams that cause the feeling of horror or fear.[114]

Déjà vu

One theory of déjà vu attributes the feeling of having previously seen or experienced something to having dreamed about a similar situation or place, and forgetting about it until one seems to be mysteriously reminded of the situation or the place while awake.[115]

See also

References

  1. ^"Dream".The American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language, Fourth Edition. 2000.Retrieved7 May2009.
  2. ^"Brain Basics: Understanding Sleep".National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke.2006.Archivedfrom the original on 11 October 2007.Retrieved16 December2007.
  3. ^Lee Ann Obringer (2006).How Dream Works.Archivedfrom the original on 18 April 2006.Retrieved4 May2006.
  4. ^Krippner, Stanley; Bogzaran, Fariba; Carvalho, Andre Percia de (2002).Extraordinary Dreams and How To Work with Them.Albany, NY: State University of New York Press. p. 9.ISBN0-7914-5257-3.Clay tablets have been found, dating to about 2500 B.C.E., that contain interpretive material for Babylonian andAssyriandreamers.
  5. ^Seligman, K (1948).Magic, Supernaturalism and Religion.New York: Random House.
  6. ^abcdBlack, Jeremy; Green, Anthony (1992).Gods, Demons and Symbols of Ancient Mesopotamia: An Illustrated Dictionary.Austin: University of Texas Press. pp. 71–72, 89–90.ISBN0714117056.
  7. ^abFreud, Sigmund(1965). James Strachey (ed.).The Interpretation of Dreams.Translated by James Strachey. New York: Avon.
  8. ^Schredl, Michael; Bohusch, Claudia; Kahl, Johanna; Mader, Andrea; Somesan, Alexandra (2000). "The Use of Dreams in Psychotherapy".The Journal of Psychotherapy Practice and Research.9(2): 81–87.
  9. ^Kavanau, J.L. (2000). "Sleep, memory maintenance, and mental disorders".Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences.12(2): 199–208.doi:10.1176/jnp.12.2.199.ISSN0895-0172.PMID11001598.
  10. ^abDodds, E. R.(1951).The Greeks and the Irrational.Berkeley: University of California Press. p. 105.The Greeks never spoke as we do ofhavinga dream, but always ofseeinga dream....
  11. ^Packer, Sharon (2002).Dreams in Myth, Medicine, and Movies.Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers. p. 85.ISBN0-275-97243-7.…[M]any more ancient cultures think that dreams are imposed by a force that resides outside the individual.
  12. ^Macrobius(1952) [430].Commentary on the Dream of Scipio.Translated byW. H. Stahl.New York: Columbia University Press. p. 90.We call a dream oracular in which a parent, or a pious or revered man, or a priest, or even a god clearly reveals what will or will not transpire, and what action to take or to avoid.
  13. ^Dodds (1951), referring to the type of dream described by Macrobius: "This last type is not, I think, at all common in our own dream-experience. But there is considerable evidence that dreams of this sort were familiar in antiquity." (p. 107).
  14. ^abKrippner, Stanley; Bogzaran, Fariba; Carvalho, André Percia de (2002).Extraordinary Dreams and How To Work with Them.Albany: State University of New York Press. p. 10.ISBN0-7914-5257-3.The Egyptian papyrus of Deral-Madineh was written about 1300 B.C.E. and gives instructions on how to obtain a dream message from a god.
  15. ^Lesku, J. A.; Meyer, L. C. R.; Fuller, A.; Maloney, S. K.; Dell'Omo, G.; Vyssotski, A. L.; Rattenborg, N. C. (2011)."Ostriches sleep like platypuses".PLOS ONE.6(8): 1–7.Bibcode:2011PLoSO...623203L.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0023203.PMC3160860.PMID21887239.
  16. ^abSolms, Mark(2000). "Dreaming and REM sleep are controlled by different brain mechanisms".Behavioral and Brain Sciences.23(6): 843–850.doi:10.1017/S0140525X00003988.PMID11515144.S2CID7264870.Dreaming and REM sleep are incompletely correlated. Between 5 and 30% of REM awakenings do not elicit dream reports; and at least 5–10% of NREM awakenings do elicit dream reports that are indistinguishable from REM....
  17. ^Bulkeley, Kelly (2008).Dreaming in the world's religions: A comparative history.NYU Press. p. 14.ISBN978-0-8147-9956-7.Do animals dream? We currently have no means of proving it one way or the other, just as we have no way to determine whether human fetuses and newborns are genuinely dreaming before they develop the ability to speak and relate their experiences.
  18. ^Damasio, Antonio(2010).Self Comes to Mind.New York: Pantheon Books. p. 289.ISBN978-0-307-37875-0.…I sympathize with Julian Jaynes's claim that something of great import may have happened to the human mind during the relatively brief interval of time between the events narrated in theIliadand those that make up theOdyssey.
  19. ^Nielsen, Tore A. (1991), "Reality Dreams and Their Effects on Spiritual Belief: A Revision of Animism Theory", in Gackenbach, Jayne; Sheikh, Anees A. (eds.),Dream Images: A Call to Mental Arms,Amityville, NY: Baywood, pp. 233–264,ISBN0-89503-056-X
  20. ^Atwan, Robert (1981). "The Interpretation of Dreams, The Origin of Consciousness, and the Birth of Tragedy".Research Communication in Psychology, Psychiatry and Behavior.6(2): 163–182.
  21. ^abcHall, C., & Van de Castle, R. (1966). The Content Analysis of Dreams. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Content Analysis ExplainedArchived12 April 2007 at theWayback Machine
  22. ^Schneider, Adam; Domhoff, G. William."The Classification and Coding of Characters".University of California at Santa Cruz.Retrieved17 July2021.
  23. ^Schredl, Michael; Ciric, Petra; Götz, Simon; Wittmann, Lutz (November 2004). "Typical Dreams: Stability and Gender Differences".The Journal of Psychology.138(6): 485–494.doi:10.3200/JRLP.138.6.485-494.PMID15612605.S2CID13554573.
  24. ^Zadra, A.,"1093: Sex dreams: what do men and women dream about?"Archived27 September 2007 at theWayback Machine,SleepVolume 30, Abstract Supplement, 2007 A376.
  25. ^"Badan Pusat Statistik" Indonesia Young Adult Reproductive Health Survey 2002–2004 "p. 27"(PDF).Archived(PDF)from the original on 9 December 2012.Retrieved4 April2013.
  26. ^"How do blind people dream? – The Body Odd".March 2012. Archived fromthe originalon 24 January 2013.Retrieved10 May2013.
  27. ^Hobson, J. Allan;Pace-Schott, Edward F.; Stickgold, Robert (2000). "Dream science 2000: A response to commentaries onDreaming and the Brain".Behavioral and Brain Sciences.23(6): 1019.doi:10.1017/S0140525X00954025.S2CID144729368.
  28. ^Chiong, Winston; Leonard, Matthew K.; Chang, Edward F. (2018)."Neurosurgical Patients as Human Research Subjects: Ethical Considerations in Intracranial Electrophysiology Research".Neurosurgery.83(1): 29–37.doi:10.1093/neuros/nyx361.PMC5777911.PMID28973530.
  29. ^abHobson, J. A., Pace-Schott, E. F., & Stickgold, R. (2000). "Dreaming and the brain: Toward a cognitive neuroscience of conscious states".Behavioral and Brain Sciences,23(6), 793–842.
  30. ^Ringach, Dario L. (30 July 2009)."The limits of fMRI".Speaking of Research.Retrieved18 July2021.
  31. ^"Policies on the Use of Animals and Humans in Research".Society for Neuroscience.Retrieved18 July2021.
  32. ^Uttal, William R.(2013).Reliability in Cognitive Neuroscience.Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. p. 4.Similarly, modern neuroscience research is increasingly showing that activation areas on the brain associated with a cognitive process are far more widely distributed than had been thought only a decade or so ago. Indeed, it now seems likely that most of the brain is active in almost any cognitive process.
  33. ^Solms, Mark(2000). "Dreaming and REM sleep are controlled by different brain mechanisms".Behavioral and Brain Sciences.23(6): 843–850, discussion 904–1121.doi:10.1017/S0140525X00003988.PMID11515144.S2CID7264870.
  34. ^Braun, A. R.; Balkin, T. J; Wesensten, N. J.; Carson, R. E.; Varga, M.; Baldwin, P.; Selbie, S.; Belenky, G.; Herscovitch, P. (1997)."Regional cerebral blood flow through the sleep-wake cycle".Brain.120.Oxford University Press: 1173–1197.doi:10.1093/brain/120.7.1173.PMID9236630.
  35. ^Kalat, James W. (2015).Biological Psychology(12th ed.). Boston: Cengage. p. 288.ISBN978-1305105409.
  36. ^Viskontas, Indre (2017).Brain Myths Exploded: Lessons from Neuroscience.Chantilly, VA: The Teaching Company. p. 393.
  37. ^Ribeiro, Sidarta (2021).The oracle of night: the history and science of dreams.Daniel Hahn, Sidarta Translation of: Ribeiro. New York.ISBN978-1-5247-4690-2.OCLC1200037413.{{cite book}}:CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  38. ^Robert, W. Der Traum als Naturnothwendigkeit erklärt. Zweite Auflage, Hamburg: Seippel, 1886.
  39. ^Freud, Sigmund(1965). James Strachey (ed.).The Interpretation of Dreams.Translated by James Strachey. New York: Avon. p. 188.The view adopted by Robert [1886, 9 f.] that the purpose of dreams is to unburden our memory of the useless impressions of daytime [cf. pp. 105 f.] is plainly no longer tenable....
  40. ^Rycroft, Charles.A Critical Dictionary of Psychoanalysis.London: Penguin Books, 1995, p. 41.
  41. ^Freud, Sigmund(1965). James Strachey (ed.).The Interpretation of Dreams.Translated by James Strachey. New York: Avon. p. 253.
  42. ^Aserinsky, Eugene;Kleitman, Nathaniel (1953). "Regularly Occurring Periods of Eye Motility, and Concomitant Phenomena, during Sleep".Science.118(3062): 273–274.Bibcode:1953Sci...118..273A.doi:10.1126/science.118.3062.273.PMID13089671.
  43. ^Smith, Robert C. (1991), "The Meaning of Dreams: A Current Warning Theory", in Gackenbach, Jayne; Sheikh, Anees A. (eds.),Dream Images: A Call to Mental Arms,Amityville, NY: Baywood, pp. 127–146,ISBN0-89503-056-X
  44. ^Hobson, J. Allan; McCarley, Robert W. (December 1977). "The Brain as a Dream State Generator: An Activation-Synthesis Hypothesis of the Dream Process".The American Journal of Psychiatry.134(12): 1335–1348.doi:10.1176/ajp.134.12.1335.PMID21570.The dream process is thus seen as having its origin in sensorimotor systems, with little or no primary ideational, volitional, or emotional content. This concept is markedly different from that of the "dream thoughts" or wishes seen by Freud as the primary stimulus for the dream.
  45. ^Benjamin, Victoria."Study Links Dreaming to Increased Memory Performance".The Harvard Crimson.Retrieved27 January2022.
  46. ^Evans, C.; Newman, E. (1964). "Dreaming: An analogy from computers".New Scientist.419:577–579.
  47. ^Crick, F.; Mitchison, G. (1983). "The function of dream sleep".Nature.304(5922): 111–114.Bibcode:1983Natur.304..111C.doi:10.1038/304111a0.PMID6866101.S2CID41500914.
  48. ^Hartmann, Ernest (1995). "Making Connections in a Safe Place: Is Dreaming Psychotherapy?".Dreaming.5(4): 213–228.doi:10.1037/h0094437.
  49. ^Revonsuo, A. (2000). "The reinterpretation of dreams: an evolutionary hypothesis of the function of dreaming".Behavioral and Brain Sciences.23(6): 877–901.doi:10.1017/S0140525X00004015.PMID11515147.S2CID145340071.
  50. ^Revonsuo, A.; Tuominen, J. (2015). "Avatars in the Machine: Dreaming as a Simulation of Social Reality".Open MIND:1–28.doi:10.15502/9783958570375.ISBN9783958570375.
  51. ^Eagleman, David M.; Vaughn, Don A. (May 2021)."The Defensive Activation Theory: REM Sleep as a Mechanism to Prevent Takeover of the Visual Cortex".Frontiers in Neuroscience.15:632853.doi:10.3389/fnins.2021.632853.PMC8176926.PMID34093109.
  52. ^"Weird dreams train us for the unexpected, says new theory".the Guardian.14 May 2021.Retrieved5 January2023.
  53. ^Hoel, Erik (14 May 2021)."The overfitted brain: Dreams evolved to assist generalization".Patterns.2(5): 100244.doi:10.1016/j.patter.2021.100244.ISSN2666-3899.PMC8134940.PMID34036289.
  54. ^Lévy-Bruhl, Lucien(1923)."Chapter III Dreams".Primitive Mentality.Translated by Lilian A. Clare. New York: Macmillan. p. 98....[I]n dreams,...man passes from the one world to the other without being aware of it. Such is in fact the ordinary idea of the dream to primitive peoples. The "soul" leaves its tenement for the time being. It frequently goes very far away; it communes with spirits or with ghosts. At the moment of awakening it returns to take its place in the body once more.
  55. ^Dunne, J. W. (1950) [1927].An Experiment with Time.London: Faber. p. 23.
  56. ^Krishnananda, Swami (16 November 1996)."The Mandukya Upanishad, Section 4".Archived fromthe originalon 9 April 2015.Retrieved26 March2015.
  57. ^Bar, Shaul (2001).A letter that has not been read: Dreams in the Hebrew Bible.Hebrew Union College Press.Retrieved4 April2013.
  58. ^Edgar, Iain (2011).The Dream in Islam: From Qur'anic Tradition to Jihadist Inspiration.Oxford: Berghahn Books. p. 178.ISBN978-0-85745-235-1.Archived fromthe originalon 29 September 2011.Retrieved9 March2012.
  59. ^abcdEdgar, Iain R.; Henig, David (September 2010)."Istikhara: The Guidance and practice of Islamic dream incubation through ethnographic comparison"(PDF).History and Anthropology.21(3): 251–262.CiteSeerX10.1.1.1012.7334.doi:10.1080/02757206.2010.496781.S2CID144463607.Archived fromthe original(PDF)on 8 November 2017.Retrieved26 October2017.
  60. ^Lactantius,De Mortibus Persecutorum44.4–6, tr. J.L. Creed,Lactantius: De Mortibus Persecutorum(Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1984), qtd. in Lenski, "Reign of Constantine" (CC), 71.
  61. ^Eusebius,Vita Constantini1.27–29; Barnes,Constantine and Eusebius,43, 306; Odahl, 105–06, 319–20.
  62. ^abYoung, S. (2003)."Dreams".The encyclopedia of South Asian Folklore.Vol. 13. p. 7. Archived fromthe originalon 25 February 2018.Retrieved24 February2018– via Indian Folklife.
  63. ^abBulkeley, Kelly (2008).Dreaming in the world's religions: A comparative history.NYU Press. pp.71–73.ISBN978-0-8147-9956-7.
  64. ^Oppenheim, L.A. (1966).Mantic Dreams in the Ancient Near Eastin G. E. Von Grunebaum & R. Caillois (Eds.),The Dream and Human Societies(pp. 341–350). London, England: Cambridge University Press.
  65. ^Nils P. Heessel:Divinatorische Texte I:... oneiromantische Omina.Harrassowitz Verlag, 2007.
  66. ^O'Neil, C.W. (1976).Dreams, culture and the individual.San Francisco: Chandler & Sharp.
  67. ^Hobson, J. A. (1988).The Dreaming Brain.Basic Books.
  68. ^McCurdy, H. G. (1946). "The history of dream theory".Psychological Review.53(4): 225–233.doi:10.1037/h0062107.PMID20998507.
  69. ^Rycroft, Charles (1979).The Innocence of Dreams.Random House.
  70. ^Cicero,De Republica,6.10
  71. ^Herodotus (1998).The Histories.Oxford University Press. p.414.
  72. ^Uluru – Kata Tjuta National Park: Tjukurpa – Anangu cultureArchived11 July 2009 at theWayback Machineenvironment.gov.au, June 23, 2006
  73. ^Tedlock, B. (1981)."Quiche Maya dream Interpretation".Ethos.9(4): 313–350.doi:10.1525/eth.1981.9.4.02a00050.
  74. ^Webb, Craig (1995)."Dreams: Practical Meaning & Applications".The DREAMS Foundation. Archived fromthe originalon 5 March 2016.Retrieved30 March2008.
  75. ^"Native American Dream Beliefs".Dream Encyclopedia. Archived fromthe originalon 15 April 2012.Retrieved10 April2012.
  76. ^Morewedge, Carey K.; Norton, Michael I. (2009)."When dreaming is believing: The (motivated) interpretation of dreams"(PDF).Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.96(2): 249–264.doi:10.1037/a0013264.PMID19159131.S2CID5706448.Archived fromthe original(PDF)on 14 November 2020.
  77. ^abHines, Terence (2003).Pseudoscience and the Paranormal.Prometheus Books. pp. 78–81.ISBN978-1-57392-979-0.
  78. ^Gilovich, Thomas (1991).How We Know What Isn't So: the fallibility of human reason in everyday life.Simon & Schuster. pp. 177–180.ISBN978-0-02-911706-4.
  79. ^"Llewellyn Worldwide – Encyclopedia: Term: Veridical Dream".www.llewellyn.com.Archived fromthe originalon 18 October 2016.Retrieved16 October2016.
  80. ^Alcock, James E. (1981).Parapsychology: Science or Magic?: a psychological perspective.Oxford: Pergamon Press.ISBN978-0-08-025773-0.viaHines, Terence (2003).Pseudoscience and the Paranormal.Prometheus Books. pp. 78–81.ISBN978-1-57392-979-0.
  81. ^Madey, Scott; Thomas Gilovich (1993). "Effects of Temporal Focus on the Recall of Expectancy-Consistent and Expectancy-Inconsistent Information".Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.65(3): 458–468.doi:10.1037/0022-3514.65.3.458.PMID8410650.viaKida, Thomas (2006).Don't Believe Everything You Think: The 6 Basic Mistakes We Make in Thinking.Prometheus Books.ISBN978-1-59102-408-8.
  82. ^"The book of the duchess".Washington State University. Archived fromthe originalon 14 November 2012.Retrieved24 May2012.
  83. ^"William Langland's The Vision Concerning Piers Plowman".The History Guide. Archived fromthe originalon 6 June 2012.Retrieved24 May2012.
  84. ^Lovecraft, Howard Phillips (1995).The Dream Cycle of H. P. Lovecraft: Dreams of Terror and Death.Ballantine Books.ISBN978-0-345-38421-8.Archivedfrom the original on 3 September 2013.
  85. ^"The Neverending Story – Book – Pictures – Video – Icons".Archived fromthe originalon 1 June 2012.Retrieved24 May2012.
  86. ^abVan Riper, A. Bowdoin (2002).Science in popular culture: a reference guide.Westport:Greenwood Press.pp. 56–57.ISBN978-0-313-31822-1.
  87. ^Van Riper, op. cit., p. 57.
  88. ^Lucid dreaming FAQArchived13 March 2007 at theWayback MachinebyThe Lucidity Instituteat Psych Web.
  89. ^Watanabe, T. (2003). "Lucid Dreaming: Its Experimental Proof and Psychological Conditions".J Int Soc Life Inf Sci.21(1).ISSN1341-9226.
  90. ^"The University of Chicago Department of Psychiatry"(PDF).5 September 1975. Archived fromthe original(PDF)on 26 April 2012.Retrieved21 October2013.
  91. ^Laberge, Stephen (2014). "Lucid dreaming: Paradoxes of dreaming consciousness".Varieties of anomalous experience: Examining the scientific evidence(2nd ed.). pp. 145–173.doi:10.1037/14258-006.ISBN978-1-4338-1529-4.S2CID152082735.
  92. ^Olson, Parmy."Saying 'Hi' Through A Dream: How The Internet Could Make Sleeping More Social".Forbes.Archivedfrom the original on 31 May 2016.Retrieved24 April2016.
  93. ^ab"The Science Behind Dreams and Nightmares".NPR.org.Archivedfrom the original on 22 August 2013.Retrieved4 April2013.
  94. ^abcWatson, David (2003). "To dream, perchance to remember: Individual differences in dream recall".Personality and Individual Differences.34(7): 1271–1286.doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00114-9.
  95. ^"Why Do Some People Always Remember Their Dreams, While Others Almost Never Do?".Discover Magazine.2019.Retrieved10 February2021.
  96. ^Herlin, Bastien; Leu-Semenescu, Smaranda; Chaumereuil, Charlotte; Arnulf, Isabelle (December 2015)."Evidence that non-dreamers do dream: a REM sleep behaviour disorder model".Journal of Sleep Research.24(6): 602–609.doi:10.1111/jsr.12323.PMID26307463.Adults report, on average, 1–2.8 dream recalls per week in a dream questionnaire and 2.38 dream recalls per week when a home dream diary is completed
  97. ^Hobson, J.A.; McCarly, R.W. (1977)."The brain as a dream-state generator: An activation-synthesis hypothesis of the dream process"(PDF).American Journal of Psychiatry.134(12): 1335–1348.doi:10.1176/ajp.134.12.1335.PMID21570.S2CID10396934.Archived fromthe original(PDF)on 9 July 2017.
  98. ^Underwood, Emily (5 April 2013)."How to Build a Dream-Reading Machine".Science.340(6128): 21.doi:10.1126/science.340.6128.21.ISSN0036-8075.PMID23559230.Retrieved15 May2023.
  99. ^Hawks, Charlotte (April 5, 2018)."How close are we to video-recording our dreams?".CNN.
  100. ^Morelle, Rebecca(4 April 2013)."Scientists 'read dreams' using brain scans".BBC News.Archivedfrom the original on 27 April 2016.Retrieved24 April2016.
  101. ^Mcguigan, F. (2012).The Psychophysiology of Thinking: Studies of Covert Processes.Elsevier.ISBN978-0-323-14700-2.
  102. ^Cvetkovic, Dean; Cosic, Irena (2011).States of Consciousness: Experimental Insights into Meditation, Waking, Sleep and Dreams.Springer Science & Business Media.ISBN978-3-642-18047-7.
  103. ^Oldis, Daniel (4 February 2016)."Can We Turn Our Dreams Into Watchable Movies?".The Huffington Post.Archivedfrom the original on 4 August 2016.Retrieved2016-08-20.
  104. ^Trang tử, tề vật luận, 12.Zhuàngzi,"Discussion on making all things equal," 12.fromZhuàngzi, Burton Watson trans.,Chuang Tzu(New York: Columbia University Press, 1996), 43.ISBN978-0-231-10595-8[1]
  105. ^Kher, Chitrarekha V. (1992).Buddhism As Presented by the Brahmanical Systems.Sri Satguru Publications.ISBN978-81-7030-293-3.
  106. ^Hajek P, Belcher M (1991). "Dream of absent-minded transgression: an empirical study of a cognitive withdrawal symptom".J Abnorm Psychol.100(4): 487–491.doi:10.1037/0021-843X.100.4.487.PMID1757662.
  107. ^"The Science of Dreaming: 9 Key Points | Psychology Today".www.psychologytoday.com.
  108. ^Dormio: A Targeted Dream Incubation Device – PMC – NCBI
  109. ^abKlinger, Eric (October 1987).Psychology Today.
  110. ^Barrett, D.L. (1979). "The Hypnotic Dream: Its Content in Comparison to Nocturnal Dreams and Waking Fantasy".Journal of Abnormal Psychology.88(5): 584–591.doi:10.1037/0021-843x.88.5.584.
  111. ^Barrett, D.L. "Fantasizers and Dissociaters: Two types of High Hypnotizables, Two Imagery Styles". in R. Kusendorf, N. Spanos, & B. Wallace (Eds.)Hypnosis and Imagination.New York: Baywood, 1996. and, Barrett, D.L. "Dissociaters, Fantasizers, and their Relation to Hypnotizability" in Barrett, D.L. (Ed.)Hypnosis and Hypnotherapy,(2 vol.): Vol. 1:History, theory and general research,Vol. 2:Psychotherapy research and applications,New York: Praeger/Greenwood, 2010.
  112. ^Tierney, John(28 June 2010)."Discovering the Virtues of a Wandering Mind".The New York Times.Archivedfrom the original on 21 April 2017.
  113. ^American Psychiatric Association (2000), Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed, TR, p. 631
  114. ^Hockenbury, Don H.; Hockenbury, Sandra E. (2010).Discovering psychology(5th ed.). New York: Worth Publishers. p. 157.ISBN978-1-4292-1650-0.
  115. ^Lohff, David C. (2004).The Dream Directory: The Comprehensive Guide to Analysis and Interpretation.Running Press.ISBN978-0-7624-1962-3.

Further reading

  • Dreaming (journal)
  • Jung, Carl(1934).The Practice of Psychotherapy."The Practical Use of Dream-analysis".New York: Routledge & Kegan Paul. pp. 139–.ISBN978-0-7100-1645-4.
  • Jung, Carl (2002).Dreams (Routledge Classics).New York: Routledge.ISBN978-0-415-26740-3.
  • Harris, William V. (2009)Dreams and Еxperience in Classical Antiquity.Cambridge, MA & London: Harvard University Press.