Fixationorvisual fixationis the maintaining of thegazeon a single location. An animal can exhibit visual fixation if it possess afoveain the anatomy of theireye.The fovea is typically located at the center of theretinaand is the point of clearest vision. The species in which fixational eye movement has been verified thus far include humans, primates, cats, rabbits, turtles, salamanders, and owls. Regulareye movementalternates betweensaccadesand visual fixations, the notable exception being insmooth pursuit,controlled by a differentneural substratethat appears to have developed for hunting prey. The term "fixation" can either be used to refer to the point in time and space of focus or the act of fixating. Fixation, in the act of fixating, is the point between any two saccades, during which the eyes are relatively stationary and virtually all visual input occurs. In the absence of retinal jitter, a laboratory condition known asretinal stabilization,perceptions tend to rapidly fade away. [1][2] To maintain visibility, thenervous systemcarries out a procedure calledfixational eye movement,which continuously stimulatesneuronsin the early visual areas of thebrainresponding totransientstimuli.There are three categories of fixational eye movement: microsaccades, ocular drifts, and ocular microtremor. At small amplitudes the boundaries between categories become unclear, particularly between drift and tremor.[3][4]

Microsaccades and Ocular Drifts

History

edit

In 1738,James Jurinmade the first known reference to a "trembling of the eye" that was presumably caused by fixational eye movements.[4]Robert Darwinnoted in 1786 that the jiggling of color after-effects was presumably the consequence of small eye movements.Eye trackingwith sufficient resolution to record fixational eye movements was developed in the 1950s. Retinal stabilization, the ability to projectstabilized imageson the retina, showed that retinal motion was necessary for visual perception, also in the 1950s. The field remained quiet until the 2000s, when key neurological properties of fixational eye movement were discovered and a new wave of research began.[5][6]

Microsaccades

edit
The lines on this image display the saccadic and microsaccadic movements of a person's eye while they looked at this face. The involuntary, micro-saccadic movement is not steady when the person's eyes are concentrated at the eyes of the woman, while the voluntary, saccadic movement goes around the periphery of the face once at any give point.

A microsaccade, also known as a "flick", is a type ofsaccade.Microsaccades are the largest and fastest of the fixational eye movements. Like saccades in general, microsaccades are usually binocular, and conjugate movements with comparable amplitudes and directions in both eyes. However, the definition of microsaccade varies from study to study and no common definition has emerged.[7]

In the 1960s, scientists suggested the maximum amplitude for microsaccades should be 12arcminutesto distinguish microsaccades and saccades.[8]However, further studies have shown that microsaccades can certainly exceed this value.[9]Newer studies have used a threshold of up to 2° to categorize microsaccades, expanding the definition by an order of magnitude. The distribution of saccade amplitudes isunimodal,giving no empirical threshold to distinguish microsaccades and saccades. Poletti et al. propose using a threshold based on the amplitude of sustained fixations and give a cutoff of 30 arcminutes or 0.5 degrees.[7]

Another way to distinguish microsaccades from saccades is by the intention of the subject when they happen. By this definition regular saccades are produced during the active and intentional exploration of the eye, during non-fixation tasks such as free viewing or visual search. Microsaccades are defined as the "involuntary saccades that occur spontaneously during intended fixation". The subjectivity of this definition has drawn criticism.[10]

Mechanism

edit

Moving in a straight-line-fashion, microsaccades have the ability to carry the retinal image from several dozen to several hundredphotoreceptorwidths. Because they shift the retinal image, microsaccades overcome adaption[8]and generate neural responses to stationary stimuli in visual neurons.[11]These movements might serve the function of maintaining visibility during fixation,[8]or might be related toattentionalshifts to objects in the visual field[12]or in memory,[13]might help limit binocularfixation disparity,[14]or may serve some combination of those functions.

Medical application

edit

Some neuroscientists believe that microsaccades are potentially important in neurological and ophthalmic diseases since they are strongly related to many features of visual perception, attention, and cognition.[15]Research aimed at finding the purpose of microsaccades began in the 1990s.[15]The development of non-invasive eye-movement-recording devices, the ability to record single-neuron activity in monkeys, and the use of computational processing power in the analysis of dynamic behavior led to advancements in microsaccade research.[11][non-primary source needed]Today, there is growing interest in research on microsaccades. Research on microsaccades includes investigating the perceptual effects of microsaccades, recording the neural responses they induce, and tracking the mechanisms behind their oculomotor generation. It has been shown that when fixation is not explicitly enforced as it often occurs in vision research experiments, microsaccades precisely shift gaze to nearby locations of interest.[16]This behavior compensates for non-uniform vision within the foveola.[17]

Some studies suggest the use of microsaccades as a diagnostic method forADHD.[18][19]Adults diagnosed with ADHD but with no medication treatments tend to blink more and make more microsaccades.[19][20]Microsaccades are also being explored as diagnostic measures forProgressive supranuclear palsy,Alzheimer's disease,autism spectrumdisorder, acute hypoxia, and other conditions.[20]

Ocular drifts

edit

Ocular drift is the fixational eye movement characterized by a smoother, slower, roaming motion of the eye when fixed on an object. The exact movement of ocular drift is often compared toBrownian motion,which is the random motion of a particle suspended in fluid as a result of its collision with the atoms and molecules that comprise that fluid. The movement can also be compared to arandom walk,characterized by random and often erratic changes in direction.[21]Ocular drifts occur incessantly during intersaccadic fixation. Although the frequency of ocular drifts is usually lower than the frequency of ocular microtremors (from 0 to 40 Hz compared to from 40 to 100 Hz), it is problematic to distinguish ocular drifts and ocular microtremors. In fact, microtremors might reflect the Brownian engine underlying the drift motion.[22]Resolution of intersaccadic eye movements is technically challenging.[6]

Brownian Motion

Mechanism

edit

The motion of ocular drift is related to the processing and encoding of space and time.[23]It is also related to acquiring minute visual details of objects that are stationary, in order for these details to be further processed.[24][25]Recent results have shown that ocular drift reformats the input signal to the retina equalizing (whitening) spatial power at non-zero temporal frequencies across a broad spatial frequency range.[26]

Medical application

edit

Ocular drift of one type was first found to be caused by an instability of the ocular motor system.[citation needed]However, more recent findings suggest that there are actually a number of hypotheses as to why ocular drifts occur. First, ocular drifts can be caused by the uncontrollable random movements driven by neuronal or muscular noise.[27]Second, ocular drifts can occur to counter controlled motor variables, namely a faulty motornegative feedbackloop.[citation needed]When the head is not immobilized, as in daily life and as is often true in eye movement recordings in the laboratory, ocular drifts compensate for the natural fixational instability of the head.[21]Ocular drifts are altered by some neurologic conditions[20]includingTourette syndrome[28]andautism spectrum disorder[29]

Ocular microtremors

edit

Ocular microtremors (OMTs) are small, quick, and synchronized oscillations of the eyes occurring at frequencies in a range of 40 to 100 Hz, although they typically occur at around 90 Hz in the average healthy individual.[citation needed]They are characterized by their high frequency and minuscule amplitude of just a fewarcseconds.Although the function of ocular microtremors is debatable and not fully known, they seem to play a role in processing of highspatial frequencies,which allows for perception of fine detail.[26][30][31]Studies show that ocular microtremors have some promise as a tool for determining the level of consciousness in an individual,[32]as well as the progression of some degenerative diseases includingParkinson's disease[33]andmultiple sclerosis.[34]

Ocular microtremor tracing with burst sections underlined

Mechanism

edit

Although originally thought to stem from spontaneous firing ofmotor units,the origin of ocular microtremors is now believed to be in the oculomotor nuclei in thereticular formationof thebrainstem.[35]This new insight opened the possibility of using ocular tremors as a gauge for neuronal activity in that region of thecentral nervous system.More research must be done, but recent studies strongly suggest that decreased activity in the brainstem correlates with decreased frequency of OMTs.[36]

Medical application

edit

Several methods of recording have been developed to observe these minuscule events, the most successful being thepiezoelectric strain gauge method,which translates eye movement through a latex probe in contact with the eye leading to piezoelectric strain gauge. This method is used in research settings; more practical adaptations of this technology have been developed for use in clinical settings to monitor the depth of anesthesia.[37]Despite the availability of these methods, tremor remains more difficult to measure than other fixational eye movements, and studies addressing medical applications of tremor movements are rare as a result.[20]Some studies have, nevertheless, pointed to the possibility that tremor movements may be useful in assessing the progression of degenerative diseases includingParkinson's disease[33]andmultiple sclerosis.[34]

See also

edit

References

edit
  1. ^Pritchard R.M.; Heron W.; Hebb D.O. (1960). "Visual Perception Approached by the Method of Stabilized Images".Canadian J. Psych.14(2): 67–77.doi:10.1037/h0083168.PMID14434966.
  2. ^Coppola, D; Purves, D (1996)."The Extraordinarily Rapid Disappearance of Entoptic Images".Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA.93(15): 8001–8004.Bibcode:1996PNAS...93.8001C.doi:10.1073/pnas.93.15.8001.PMC38864.PMID8755592.
  3. ^Rucci M., Poletti M. (2015)."Control and function of fixational eye movements".Annual Review of Vision Science.11:499–518.doi:10.1146/annurev-vision-082114-035742.PMC5082990.PMID27795997.
  4. ^abAlexander, R. G.; Martinez-Conde, S. (2019). "Fixational Eye Movements".Eye Movement Research.Studies in Neuroscience, Psychology and Behavioral Economics. pp. 73–115.doi:10.1007/978-3-030-20085-5_3.ISBN978-3-030-20083-1.{{cite book}}:|journal=ignored (help)
  5. ^Rucci M., Poletti M. (2015)."Control and function of fixational eye movements".Annual Review of Vision Science.1:499–518.doi:10.1146/annurev-vision-082114-035742.PMC5082990.PMID27795997.It is now known that these movements modulate neural responses in various cortical areas.
  6. ^abRucci, Michele;McGraw, Paul V.; Krauzlis, Richard J. (2016-01-01)."Fixational eye movements and perception".Vision Research.118:1–4.doi:10.1016/j.visres.2015.12.001.ISSN1878-5646.PMC11298813.PMID26686666.S2CID5510697.After a period of quiescence at the end of last millennium, the study of fixational eye movements has now gained wide popularity among vision scientists.
  7. ^abPoletti M., Rucci M. (2016)."A compact field guide to the study of microsaccades: Challenges and functions".Vision Research.118:83–97.doi:10.1016/j.visres.2015.01.018.PMC4537412.PMID25689315.
  8. ^abcCollewijn, Han; Kowler, Eileen (2008-01-01)."The significance of microsaccades for vision and oculomotor control".Journal of Vision.8(14): 20.1–21.doi:10.1167/8.14.20.ISSN1534-7362.PMC3522523.PMID19146321.
  9. ^Troncoso, Xoana G.; Macknik, Stephen L.; Martinez-Conde, Susana (2008-01-01)."Microsaccades counteract perceptual filling-in".Journal of Vision.8(14): 15.1–9.doi:10.1167/8.14.15.ISSN1534-7362.PMID19146316.
  10. ^Poletti M., Rucci M. (2016)."A compact field guide to the study of microsaccades: Challenges and functions".Vision Research.118:83–97.doi:10.1016/j.visres.2015.01.018.PMC4537412.PMID25689315.[...] this definition implicitly comes with drawbacks: its dependence on the subject's intention (note the terms: "involuntary", "spontaneously", "intended" ) makes it little objective and prone to different interpretations
  11. ^abMartinez-Conde, S.;Macknik, S. L.; Hubel, D. H. (2000-03-01). "Microsaccadic eye movements and firing of single cells in the striate cortex of macaque monkeys".Nature Neuroscience.3(3): 251–258.doi:10.1038/72961.ISSN1097-6256.PMID10700257.S2CID8846976.
  12. ^Laubrock; Engbert; Kliegl (2005)."Microsaccade dynamics during covert attention".Vision Research.45(6): 721–730.doi:10.1016/j.visres.2004.09.029.PMID15639499.S2CID374682.
  13. ^Martinez-Conde, S; Alexander, R (2019). "A gaze bias in the mind's eye".Nature Human Behaviour.3(5): 424–425.doi:10.1038/s41562-019-0546-1.PMID31089295.S2CID71148025.
  14. ^Valsecchi, Matteo; Gegenfurtner, Karl R. (2015)."Control of binocular gaze in a high-precision manual task".Vision Research.110(Pt B): 203–214.doi:10.1016/j.visres.2014.09.005.PMID25250983.
  15. ^abMartinez-Conde, Susana;Macknik, Stephen L.; Troncoso, Xoana G.; Hubel, David H. (2009-09-01). "Microsaccades: a neurophysiological analysis".Trends in Neurosciences.32(9): 463–475.CiteSeerX10.1.1.493.7537.doi:10.1016/j.tins.2009.05.006.ISSN1878-108X.PMID19716186.S2CID124353.
  16. ^Ko H.-K.; Poletti M.; Rucci M. (2010)."Microsaccades precisely relocate gaze in a high visual acuity task".Nat. Neurosci.13(12): 1549–1553.doi:10.1038/nn.2663.PMC3058801.PMID21037583.
  17. ^Poletti M.; Listorti C.; Rucci M. (2013)."Microscopic eye movements compensate for nonhomogeneous vision within the fovea".Curr. Biol.23(17): 1691–1695.Bibcode:2013CBio...23.1691P.doi:10.1016/j.cub.2013.07.007.PMC3881259.PMID23954428.
  18. ^Panagiotidi, M; Overton, P; Stafford, T (2017)."Increased microsaccade rate in individuals with ADHD traits".Journal of Eye Movement Research.10(1): 1–9.doi:10.16910/10.1.6.PMC7141051.PMID33828642.
  19. ^abFried; Tsitsiashvili; Bonneh; Sterkin; Wygnanski-Jaffe; Epstein (2014)."ADHD subjects fail to suppress eye blinks and microsaccades while anticipating visual stimuli but recover with medication".Vision Res.101:62–72.doi:10.1016/j.visres.2014.05.004.PMID24863585.
  20. ^abcdAlexander, Robert; Macknik, Stephen; Martinez-Conde, Susana (2018)."Microsaccade Characteristics in Neurological and Ophthalmic Disease".Frontiers in Neurology.9(144): 144.doi:10.3389/fneur.2018.00144.PMC5859063.PMID29593642.
  21. ^abPoletti M.; Aytekin M.; Rucci M. (2015)."Head-eye coordination at a microscopic scale".Current Biology.25(24): 3253–3259.Bibcode:2015CBio...25.3253P.doi:10.1016/j.cub.2015.11.004.PMC4733666.PMID26687623.
  22. ^Ahissar, Ehud; Arieli, Amos; Fried, Moshe; Bonneh, Yoram (2016-01-01). "On the possible roles of microsaccades and drifts in visual perception".Vision Research.118:25–30.doi:10.1016/j.visres.2014.12.004.ISSN1878-5646.PMID25535005.S2CID12194501.
  23. ^Ahissar E., Arieli A. (2001)."Figuring space by time".Neuron.32(2): 185–201.doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00466-4.PMID11683990.
  24. ^Kuang, Xutao; Poletti, Martina; Victor, Jonathan D.;Rucci, Michele(2012-03-20)."Temporal encoding of spatial information during active visual fixation".Current Biology.22(6): 510–514.Bibcode:2012CBio...22..510K.doi:10.1016/j.cub.2012.01.050.ISSN1879-0445.PMC3332095.PMID22342751.
  25. ^Ahissar E., Arieli A. (2012)."Seeing via miniature eye movements: A dynamic hypothesis for vision".Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience.6:89.doi:10.3389/fncom.2012.00089.PMC3492788.PMID23162458.
  26. ^abRucci M., Victor J. D. (2015)."The unsteady eye: an information processing stage, not a bug".Trends in Neurosciences.38(4): 195–206.doi:10.1016/j.tins.2015.01.005.PMC4385455.PMID25698649.
  27. ^Carpenter, R. H. S. (1988).Movements of the Eyes(2nd ed.). London: Pion.
  28. ^Shaikh (2017)."Fixational eye movements in Tourette syndrome".Neurological Sciences.38(11): 1977–1984.doi:10.1007/s10072-017-3069-4.PMC6246774.PMID28815321.
  29. ^Frey, Hans-Peter; Molholm, Sophie; Lalor, Edmund C; Russo, Natalie N; Foxe, John J (2013)."Atypical cortical representation of peripheral visual space in children with an autism spectrum disorder".European Journal of Neuroscience.38(1): 2125–2138.doi:10.1111/ejn.12243.PMC4587666.PMID23692590.
  30. ^Rucci, Michele; Iovin, Ramon; Poletti, Martina; Santini, Fabrizio (2007). "Miniature eye movements enhance fine spatial detail".Nature.447(7146): 852–855.Bibcode:2007Natur.447..852R.doi:10.1038/nature05866.PMID17568745.S2CID4416740.
  31. ^Otero-Millan, Jorge; Macknik, Stephen L.; Martinez-Conde, Susana (2014-01-01)."Fixational eye movements and binocular vision".Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience.8:52.doi:10.3389/fnint.2014.00052.ISSN1662-5145.PMC4083562.PMID25071480.
  32. ^Bolger, C; Sheahan, N; Coakley, D; Malone, J (1992). "High frequency eye tremor: reliability of measurement".Clinical Physics and Physiological Measurement.13(2): 151–9.Bibcode:1992CPPM...13..151B.doi:10.1088/0143-0815/13/2/007.PMID1499258.
  33. ^abBolger, C.; Bojanic, S.; Sheahan, N. F.; Coakley, D.; Malone, J. F. (1999-04-01)."Ocular microtremor in patients with idiopathic Parkinson's disease".Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry.66(4): 528–531.doi:10.1136/jnnp.66.4.528.ISSN0022-3050.PMC1736284.PMID10201430.
  34. ^abBolger, C.; Bojanic, S.; Sheahan, N.; Malone, J.; Hutchinson, M.; Coakley, D. (2000-05-01)."Ocular microtremor (OMT): a new neurophysiological approach to multiple sclerosis".Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry.68(5): 639–642.doi:10.1136/jnnp.68.5.639.ISSN0022-3050.PMC1736931.PMID10766897.
  35. ^Spauschus, A.; Marsden, J.; Halliday, D. M.; Rosenberg, J. R.; Brown, P. (1999-06-01). "The origin of ocular microtremor in man".Experimental Brain Research.126(4): 556–562.doi:10.1007/s002210050764.ISSN0014-4819.PMID10422719.S2CID2472268.
  36. ^Bojanic, S.; Simpson, T.; Bolger, C. (2001-04-01)."Ocular microtremor: a tool for measuring depth of anaesthesia?".British Journal of Anaesthesia.86(4): 519–522.doi:10.1093/bja/86.4.519.ISSN0007-0912.PMID11573625.
  37. ^Bengi, H.; Thomas, J. G. (1968-03-01). "Three electronic methods for recording ocular tremor".Medical & Biological Engineering.6(2): 171–179.doi:10.1007/bf02474271.ISSN0025-696X.PMID5651798.S2CID29028883.