Pitchis aperceptualproperty that allowssoundsto be ordered on afrequency-relatedscale,[1] or more commonly, pitch is the quality that makes it possible to judge sounds as "higher" and "lower" in the sense associated with musicalmelodies.[2] Pitch is a majorauditoryattribute ofmusical tones,along withduration,loudness,andtimbre.[3]

In musical notation, the different vertical positions of notes indicate differentpitches.
Play top:
Play bottom:

Pitch may be quantified as afrequency,but pitch is not a purely objective physical property; it is a subjectivepsychoacousticalattribute of sound. Historically, the study of pitch and pitch perception has been a central problem in psychoacoustics, and has been instrumental in forming and testing theories of sound representation, processing, and perception in the auditory system.[4]

Perception

edit

Pitch and frequency

edit

Pitch is an auditory sensation in which a listener assignsmusical tonesto relative positions on amusical scalebased primarily on their perception of thefrequencyof vibration (audio frequency).[5]Pitch is closely related to frequency, but the two are not equivalent. Frequency is an objective, scientific attribute which can be measured. Pitch is the subjective perception of a sound wave by the individual person, which cannot be directly measured. However, this does not necessarily mean that people will not agree on which notes are higher and lower.

Theoscillationsof sound waves can often be characterized in terms offrequency.Pitchesare usually associated with, and thus quantified as,frequencies(in cycles per second, or hertz), by comparing the sounds being assessed against sounds withpure tones(ones withperiodic,sinusoidalwaveforms). Complex and aperiodic sound waves can often be assigned apitchby this method.[6][7][8]

According to theAmerican National Standards Institute,pitch is the auditory attribute of sound allowing those sounds to be ordered on a scale from low to high. Since pitch is such a closeproxyfor frequency, it is almost entirely determined by how quickly the sound wave is making the air vibrate and has almost nothing to do with the intensity, oramplitude,of the wave. That is, "high" pitch means very rapid oscillation, and "low" pitch corresponds to slower oscillation. Despite that, theidiomrelating vertical height to sound pitch is shared by most languages.[9]At least in English, it is just one of many deep conceptual metaphors that involve up/down. The exact etymological history of the musical sense of high and low pitch is still unclear. There is evidence that humans do actually perceive that the source of a sound is slightly higher or lower in vertical space when the sound frequency is increased or reduced.[9]

In most cases, the pitch of complex sounds such asspeechandmusical notescorresponds very nearly to the repetition rate of periodic or nearly-periodic sounds, or to thereciprocalof the time interval between repeating similar events in the sound waveform.[7][8]

The pitch of complex tones can be ambiguous, meaning that two or more different pitches can be perceived, depending upon the observer.[4]When the actualfundamental frequencycan be precisely determined through physical measurement, it may differ from the perceived pitch because ofovertones,also known as upper partials,harmonicor otherwise. A complex tone composed of two sine waves of 1000 and 1200 Hz may sometimes be heard as up to three pitches: two spectral pitches at 1000 and 1200 Hz, derived from the physical frequencies of the pure tones, and thecombination toneat 200 Hz, corresponding to the repetition rate of the waveform. In a situation like this, the percept at 200 Hz is commonly referred to as themissing fundamental,which is often thegreatest common divisorof the frequencies present.[10]

Pitch depends to a lesser degree on thesound pressurelevel (loudness, volume) of the tone, especially at frequencies below 1,000 Hz and above 2,000 Hz. The pitch of lower tones gets lower as sound pressure increases. For instance, a tone of 200 Hz that is very loud seems one semitone lower in pitch than if it is just barely audible. Above 2,000 Hz, the pitch gets higher as the sound gets louder.[11]These results were obtained in the pioneering works by S. Stevens[12]and W. Snow.[13]Later investigations, e.g. by A. Cohen, have shown that in most cases the apparent pitch shifts were not significantly different from pitch‐matching errors. When averaged, the remaining shifts followed the directions of Stevens's curves but were small (2% or less by frequency, i.e. not more than a semitone).[14]

Theories of pitch perception

edit

Theoriesof pitch perception try to explain how the physical sound and specific physiology of the auditory system work together to yield the experience of pitch. In general, pitch perception theories can be divided intoplace codingandtemporal coding.Place theory holds that the perception of pitch is determined by the place of maximum excitation on thebasilar membrane.

A place code, taking advantage of thetonotopyin the auditory system, must be in effect for the perception of high frequencies, since neurons have an upper limit on how fast they canphase-locktheiraction potentials.[5]However, a purely place-based theory cannot account for the accuracy of pitch perception in the low and middle frequency ranges. Moreover, there is some evidence that some non-human primates lack auditory cortex responses to pitch despite having clear tonotopic maps in auditory cortex, showing that tonotopic place codes are not sufficient for pitch responses.[15]

Temporal theories offer an alternative that appeals to the temporal structure of action potentials, mostly thephase-lockof action potentials to frequencies in a stimulus. The precise way this temporal structure helps code for pitch at higher levels is still debated, but the processing seems to be based on anautocorrelationof action potentials in the auditory nerve.[16]However, it has long been noted that a neural mechanism that may accomplish a delay—a necessary operation of a true autocorrelation—has not been found.[5]At least one model shows that a temporal delay is unnecessary to produce an autocorrelation model of pitch perception, appealing tophase shiftsbetween cochlear filters;[17]however, earlier work has shown that certain sounds with a prominent peak in their autocorrelation function do not elicit a corresponding pitch percept,[18][19]and that certain sounds without a peak in their autocorrelation function nevertheless elicit a pitch.[20][21]To be a more complete model, autocorrelation must therefore apply to signals that represent the output of thecochlea,as via auditory-nerve interspike-interval histograms.[19]Some theories of pitch perception hold that pitch has inherentoctaveambiguities, and therefore is best decomposed into a pitchchroma,a periodic value around the octave, like the note names in Western music—and a pitchheight,which may be ambiguous, that indicates the octave the pitch is in.[4]

Just-noticeable difference

edit

Thejust-noticeable difference (jnd)(thethresholdat which a change is perceived) depends on the tone's frequency content. Below 500 Hz, the jnd is about 3 Hz for sine waves, and 1 Hz for complex tones; above 1000 Hz, the jnd for sine waves is about 0.6% (about 10cents).[22] Thejndis typically tested by playing two tones in quick succession with the listener asked if there was a difference in their pitches.[11]Thejndbecomes smaller if the two tones are playedsimultaneouslyas the listener is then able to discernbeat frequencies.The total number of perceptible pitch steps in the human hearing range is about 1,400; the total number of notes in the equal-tempered scale, from 16 to 16,000 Hz, is 120.[11]

Aural illusions

edit

The relative perception of pitch can be fooled, resulting inaural illusions.There are several of these, such as thetritone paradox,but most notably theShepard scale,where a continuous or discrete sequence of specially formed tones can be made to sound as if the sequence continues ascending or descending forever.

Definite and indefinite pitch

edit

Not all musical instruments make notes with a clear pitch. Theunpitched percussion instruments(a class ofpercussion instruments) do not produce particular pitches. A sound or note ofdefinite pitchis one where a listener can possibly (or relatively easily) discern the pitch. Sounds with definite pitch haveharmonicfrequency spectraor close to harmonic spectra.[11]

A sound generated on any instrument produces many modes of vibration that occur simultaneously. A listener hears numerous frequencies at once. The vibration with the lowest frequency is called thefundamental frequency;the other frequencies areovertones.[23] Harmonicsare an important class of overtones with frequencies that are integer multiples of the fundamental. Whether or not the higher frequencies are integer multiples, they are collectively called thepartials,referring to the different parts that make up the total spectrum.

A sound or note ofindefinite pitchis one that a listener finds impossible or relatively difficult to identify as to pitch. Sounds with indefinite pitch do not have harmonic spectra or have altered harmonic spectra—a characteristic known asinharmonicity.

It is still possible for two sounds of indefinite pitch to clearly be higher or lower than one another. For instance, asnare drumsounds higher pitched than abass drumthough both have indefinite pitch, because its sound contains higher frequencies. In other words, it is possible and often easy to roughly discern the relative pitches of two sounds of indefinite pitch, but sounds of indefinite pitch do not neatly correspond to any specific pitch.

Pitch standards and standard pitch

edit

A pitch standard (alsoconcert pitch) is the conventional pitch reference thatmusical instrumentsin a group are tuned to for a performance. Concert pitch may vary from ensemble to ensemble, and has varied widely over musical history.

Standard pitch is a more widely accepted convention. TheAabovemiddle Cis usually set at 440 Hz (often written as "A =440 Hz"or sometimes" A440 "), although other frequencies, such as 442 Hz, are also often used as variants. Another standard pitch, the so-calledBaroque pitch,has been set in the 20th century as A = 415 Hz—approximately an equal-tempered semitone lower than A440 to facilitate transposition. TheClassical pitchcan be set to either 427 Hz (about halfway between A415 and A440) or 430 Hz (also between A415 and A440 but slightly sharper than the quarter tone). And ensembles specializing inauthentic performanceset the A above middle C to 432 Hz or 435 Hz when performing repertoire from theRomanticera.

Transposing instrumentshave their origin in the variety of pitch standards. In modern times, they conventionally have theirpartstransposed into differentkeysfrom voices and other instruments (and even from each other). As a result, musicians need a way to refer to a particular pitch in an unambiguous manner when talking to each other.

For example, the most common type ofclarinetortrumpet,when playing a note written in theirpartas C, sounds a pitch that is called Bon a non-transposing instrument like a violin (which indicates that at one time these wind instruments played at a standard pitch a tone lower than violin pitch). To refer to that pitch unambiguously, a musician calls itconcert B,meaning, "the pitch that someone playing a non-transposing instrument like a violin calls B."

Labeling pitches

edit
Note frequencies, four-octave C major diatonic scale, starting withC1.

Pitches are labeled using:

For example, one might refer to the A above middle C asa′,A4,or440 Hz.In standard Westernequal temperament,the notion of pitch is insensitive to "spelling": the description "G4double sharp "refers to the same pitch asA4;in other temperaments, these may be distinct pitches. Human perception of musical intervals is approximately logarithmic with respect tofundamental frequency:the perceived interval between the pitches "A220" and "A440" is the same as the perceived interval between the pitchesA440andA880.Motivated by this logarithmic perception, music theorists sometimes represent pitches using a numerical scale based on the logarithm of fundamental frequency. For example, one can adopt the widely usedMIDIstandard to map fundamental frequency,f,to a real number,p,as follows.

This creates a linearpitch spacein which octaves have size 12, semitones (the distance between adjacent keys on the piano keyboard) have size 1, and A440 is assigned the number 69. (SeeFrequencies of notes.) Distance in this space corresponds to musical intervals as understood by musicians. An equal-tempered semitone is subdivided into 100cents.The system is flexible enough to include "microtones" not found on standard piano keyboards. For example, the pitch halfway between C (60) and C(61) can be labeled 60.5.

The following table shows frequencies in Hertz for notes in various octaves, named according to the"German method" of octave nomenclature:

Note Sub-contra Contra Great Small One-lined Two-lined Three-lined Four-lined Five-lined
B/C 16.35 32.70 65.41 130.81 261.63 523.25 1046.50 2093.00 4186.01
C/D 17.32 34.65 69.30 138.59 277.18 554.37 1108.73 2217.46 4434.92
D 18.35 36.71 73.42 146.83 293.66 587.33 1174.66 2349.32 4698.64
D/E 19.45 38.89 77.78 155.56 311.13 622.25 1244.51 2489.02 4978.03
E/F 20.60 41.20 82.41 164.81 329.63 659.26 1318.51 2637.02 5274.04
E/F 21.83 43.65 87.31 174.61 349.23 698.46 1396.91 2793.83 5587.65
F/G 23.12 46.25 92.50 185.00 369.99 739.99 1479.98 2959.96 5919.91
G 24.50 49.00 98.00 196.00 392.00 783.99 1567.99 3135.96 6271.93
G/A 25.96 51.91 103.83 207.65 415.30 830.61 1661.22 3322.44 6644.88
A 27.50 55.00 110.00 220.00 440.00 880.00 1760.00 3520.00 7040.00
A/B 29.14 58.27 116.54 233.08 466.16 932.33 1864.66 3729.31 7458.62
B/C 30.87 61.74 123.47 246.94 493.88 987.77 1975.53 3951.07 7902.13

Scales

edit

The relative pitches of individual notes in ascalemay be determined by one of a number oftuning systems.In the west, the twelve-notechromatic scaleis the most common method of organization, withequal temperamentnow the most widely used method of tuning that scale. In it, the pitch ratio between any two successive notes of the scale is exactly the twelfth root of two (or about 1.05946). Inwell-temperedsystems (as used in the time ofJohann Sebastian Bach,for example), different methods ofmusical tuningwere used.

In almost all of these systemsintervalof theoctavedoubles the frequency of a note; for example, an octave aboveA440is 880 Hz. If however the firstovertoneis sharp due toinharmonicity,as in the extremes of the piano,tunersresort tooctave stretching.

Other musical meanings of pitch

edit

Inatonal,twelve tone,ormusical set theory,a "pitch" is a specific frequency while apitch classis all the octaves of a frequency. In many analytic discussions of atonal and post-tonal music, pitches are named withintegersbecause of octave and enharmonic equivalency (for example, in a serial system, Cand Dare considered the same pitch, while C4and C5are functionally the same, one octave apart).

Discrete pitches, rather than continuously variable pitches, are virtually universal, with exceptions including "tumbling strains"[26]and "indeterminate-pitch chants".[27]Gliding pitches are used in most cultures, but are related to the discrete pitches they reference or embellish.[28]

See also

edit

References

edit
  1. ^Anssi Klapuri, "Introduction to Music Transcription",inSignal Processing Methods for Music Transcription,edited by Anssi Klapuri and Manuel Davy, 1–20 (New York: Springer, 2006): p. 8.ISBN978-0-387-30667-4.
  2. ^ Plack, Christopher J.; Andrew J. Oxenham; Richard R. Fay, eds. (2005).Pitch: Neural Coding and Perception.New York: Springer.ISBN978-0-387-23472-4.For the purposes of this book we decided to take a conservative approach, and to focus on the relationship between pitch and musical melodies. Following the earlier ASA definition, we define pitch as 'that attribute of sensation whose variation is associated with musical melodies.' Although some might find this too restrictive, an advantage of this definition is that it provides a clear procedure for testing whether or not a stimulus evokes a pitch, and a clear limitation on the range of stimuli that we need to consider in our discussions.
  3. ^ Roy D. Patterson; Etienne Gaudrain & Thomas C. Walters (2010)."The Perception of Family and Register in Musical Tones".In Mari Riess Jones; Richard R. Fay & Arthur N. Popper (eds.).Music Perception.Springer. pp. 37–38.ISBN978-1-4419-6113-6.
  4. ^abcHartmann, William Morris (1997).Signals, Sound, and Sensation.Springer. pp. 145, 284, 287.ISBN978-1-56396-283-7.
  5. ^abc Plack, Christopher J.; Andrew J. Oxenham; Richard R. Fay, eds. (2005).Pitch: Neural Coding and Perception.Springer.ISBN978-0-387-23472-4.
  6. ^ Robert A. Dobie & Susan B. Van Hemel (2005).Hearing Loss: Determining Eligibility for Social Security Benefits.National Academies Press. pp. 50–51.ISBN978-0-309-09296-8.
  7. ^ab E. Bruce Goldstein (2001).Blackwell Handbook of Perception(4th ed.). Wiley-Blackwell. p. 381.ISBN978-0-631-20683-5.
  8. ^ab Richard Lyon & Shihab Shamma (1996)."Auditory Representation of Timbre and Pitch".In Harold L. Hawkins & Teresa A. McMullen (eds.).Auditory Computation.Springer. pp. 221–23.ISBN978-0-387-97843-7.
  9. ^abCarroll C. Pratt, "The Spatial Character of High and Low Tones",Journal of Experimental Psychology13 (1930): 278–85.
  10. ^Schwartz, David A.; Dale Purves (May 2004). "Pitch Is Determined by Naturally Occurring Periodic Sounds".Hearing Research.194(1–2): 31–46.doi:10.1016/j.heares.2004.01.019.PMID15276674.S2CID40608136.
  11. ^abcdOlson, Harry F.(1967).Music, Physics and Engineering.Dover Publications. pp. 171, 248–251.ISBN978-0-486-21769-7.
  12. ^Stevens S. S. The relation of pitch to intensity//J. Acoust. Soc. Amer. 1935. Vol. 6. p. 150–154.
  13. ^Snow W. B. (1936) Change of Pitch with Loudness at Low Frequencies. J. Acoust. Soc. Am/ 8:14–19.
  14. ^Cohen, A. (1961). Further investigation of the effects of intensity upon the pitch of pure tones. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 33, 1363–1376.https://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.1908441
  15. ^Norman-Haignere, S.V.; Kanwisher, N.G.; McDermott, J.; Conway, B.R. (10 June 2019)."Divergence in the functional organization of human and macaque auditory cortex revealed by fMRI responses to harmonic tones".Nature Neuroscience.22(7): 1057–1060.doi:10.1038/s41593-019-0410-7.ISSN1097-6256.PMC6592717.PMID31182868.
  16. ^Cariani, P.A.; Delgutte, B. (September 1996)."Neural Correlates of the Pitch of Complex Tones. I. Pitch and Pitch Salience"(PDF).Journal of Neurophysiology.76(3): 1698–1716.doi:10.1152/jn.1996.76.3.1698.PMID8890286.Retrieved13 November2012.
  17. ^Cheveigné, A. de; Pressnitzer, D. (June 2006)."The Case of the Missing Delay Lines: Synthetic Delays Obtained by Cross-channel Phase Interaction"(PDF).Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.119(6): 3908–3918.Bibcode:2006ASAJ..119.3908D.doi:10.1121/1.2195291.PMID16838534.Retrieved13 November2012.
  18. ^Kaernbach, C.; Demany, L. (October 1998). "Psychophysical Evidence Against the Autocorrelation Theory of Auditory Temporal Processing".Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.104(4): 2298–2306.Bibcode:1998ASAJ..104.2298K.doi:10.1121/1.423742.PMID10491694.
  19. ^abPressnitzer, D.; Cheveigné, A. de; Winter, I.M. (January 2002)."Perceptual Pitch Shift for Sounds with Similar Waveform Autocorrelation".Acoustics Research Letters Online.3(1): 1–6.doi:10.1121/1.1416671.
  20. ^Burns, E.M.; Viemeister, N. F. (October 1976). "Nonspectral Pitch".Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.60(4): 863–69.Bibcode:1976ASAJ...60..863B.doi:10.1121/1.381166.
  21. ^Fitzgerald, M. B.; Wright, B. (December 2005). "A Perceptual Learning Investigation of the Pitch Elicited by Amplitude-Modulated Noise".Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.118(6): 3794–3803.Bibcode:2005ASAJ..118.3794F.doi:10.1121/1.2074687.PMID16419824.
  22. ^ Birger Kollmeier; Thomas Brand & B. Meyer (2008)."Perception of Speech and Sound".In Jacob Benesty; M. Mohan Sondhi & Yiteng Huang (eds.).Springer Handbook of Speech Processing.Springer. p. 65.ISBN978-3-540-49125-5.
  23. ^Levitin, Daniel (2007).This Is Your Brain on Music.New York: Penguin Group. p. 40.ISBN978-0-452-28852-2.The one with the slowest vibration rate—the one lowest in pitch—is referred to as the fundamental frequency, and the others are collectively called overtones.
  24. ^The Concise Grove Dictionary of Music:Hermann von Helmholtz,Oxford University Press (1994),Answers.com.Retrieved 3 August 2007.
  25. ^Helmholtz, Hermann (1885).On the Sensations of Tone (English Translation).Cosimo. p. 15.ISBN9781602066397.
  26. ^Sachs, C. and Kunst, J. (1962). InThe Wellsprings of Music,edited by J. Kunst. The Hague: Marinus Nijhoff. Cited in Burns (1999).
  27. ^Malm, W.P. (1967).Music Cultures of the Pacific, the Near East, and Asia.Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Cited in Burns (1999).
  28. ^Burns, Edward M. (1999). "Intervals, Scales, and Tuning",The Psychology of Music,second edition. Deutsch, Diana, ed. San Diego: Academic Press.ISBN0-12-213564-4.

Further reading

edit
  • Moore, B.C. & Glasberg, B.R. (1986) "Thresholds for Hearing Mistuned Partials as Separate Tones in Harmonic Complexes".Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,80, 479–83.
  • Parncutt, R. (1989).Harmony: A Psychoacoustical Approach.Berlin: Springer-Verlag, 1989.
  • Schneider, P.; Sluming, V.; Roberts, N.; Scherg, M.; Goebel, R.; Specht, H.-J.; Dosch, H.G.; Bleeck, S.; Stippich, C.; Rupp, A. (2005). "Structural and Functional Asymmetry of Lateral Heschl's Gyrus Reflects Pitch Perception Preference".Nat. Neurosci.[full citation needed]8, 1241–47.
  • Terhardt, E., Stoll, G. and Seewann, M. (1982). "Algorithm for Extraction of Pitch and Pitch Salience from Complex Tonal Signals".Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,71, 679–88.
edit