Thevisual systemis the physiological basis ofvisual perception(the ability todetect and processlight). The system detects,transducesand interprets information concerninglightwithin thevisible rangeto construct animageand build amental modelof the surrounding environment. The visual system is associated with theeyeand functionally divided into theopticalsystem (includingcorneaandlens) and theneuralsystem (including theretinaandvisual cortex).
Visual system | |
---|---|
Identifiers | |
FMA | 7191 |
Anatomical terminology |
The visual system performs a number of complex tasks based on theimage formingfunctionality of the eye, including the formation of monocular images, the neural mechanisms underlyingstereopsisand assessment of distances to (depth perception) and between objects,motion perception,pattern recognition,accuratemotor coordinationunder visual guidance, andcolour vision.Together, these facilitate higher order tasks, such asobject identification.Theneuropsychologicalside of visual information processing is known asvisual perception,an abnormality of which is calledvisual impairment,and a complete absence of which is calledblindness.The visual system also has several non-image forming visual functions, independent of visual perception, including thepupillary light reflexand circadianphotoentrainment.
This article describes the human visual system, which is representative ofmammalian vision,and to a lesser extent thevertebratevisual system.
System overview
editOptical
editTogether, thecorneaandlensrefract light into a small image and shine it on theretina.The retinatransducesthis image into electrical pulses usingrodsandcones.Theoptic nervethen carries these pulses through theoptic canal.Upon reaching theoptic chiasmthe nerve fibers decussate (left becomes right). The fibers then branch and terminate in three places.[1][2][3][4][5][6][7]
Neural
editMost of the optic nerve fibers end in thelateral geniculate nucleus(LGN). Before the LGN forwards the pulses to V1 of the visual cortex (primary) it gauges the range of objects and tags every major object with a velocity tag. These tags predict object movement.
The LGN also sends some fibers to V2 and V3.[8][9][10][11][12]
V1 performs edge-detection to understand spatial organization (initially, 40 milliseconds in, focusing on even small spatial and color changes. Then, 100 milliseconds in, upon receiving the translated LGN, V2, and V3 info, also begins focusing on global organization). V1 also creates a bottom-upsaliency mapto guide attention orgaze shift.[13]
V2 both forwards (direct and viapulvinar) pulses to V1 and receives them. Pulvinar is responsible forsaccadeand visual attention. V2 serves much the same function as V1, however, it also handlesillusory contours,determining depth by comparing left and right pulses (2D images), and foreground distinguishment. V2 connects to V1 - V5.
V3 helps process 'global motion' (direction and speed) of objects. V3 connects to V1 (weak), V2, and theinferior temporal cortex.[14][15]
V4 recognizes simple shapes, and gets input from V1 (strong), V2, V3, LGN, and pulvinar.[16]V5's outputs include V4 and its surrounding area, and eye-movement motor cortices (frontal eye-fieldandlateral intraparietal area).
V5's functionality is similar to that of the other V's, however, it integrates local object motion into global motion on a complex level. V6 works in conjunction with V5 on motion analysis. V5 analyzes self-motion, whereas V6 analyzes motion of objects relative to the background. V6's primary input is V1, with V5 additions. V6 houses thetopographical mapfor vision. V6 outputs to the region directly around it (V6A). V6A has direct connections to arm-moving cortices, including thepremotor cortex.[17][18]
Theinferior temporal gyrusrecognizes complex shapes, objects, and faces or, in conjunction with thehippocampus,creates newmemories.[19]Thepretectal areais seven uniquenuclei.Anterior, posterior and medial pretectal nuclei inhibit pain (indirectly), aid inREM,and aid theaccommodation reflex,respectively.[20]TheEdinger-Westphal nucleusmoderatespupil dilationand aids (since it provides parasympathetic fibers) in convergence of the eyes and lens adjustment.[21]Nuclei of the optic tract are involved in smooth pursuit eye movement and the accommodation reflex, as well as REM.
Thesuprachiasmatic nucleusis the region of thehypothalamusthat halts production ofmelatonin(indirectly) at first light.[22]
Structure
edit- Theeye,especially theretina
- Theoptic nerve
- Theoptic chiasma
- Theoptic tract
- Thelateral geniculate body
- Theoptic radiation
- Thevisual cortex
- Thevisual association cortex.
These are components of thevisual pathway,also called theoptic pathway,[23]that can be divided intoanterior and posterior visual pathways.The anterior visual pathway refers to structures involved in vision before thelateral geniculate nucleus.The posterior visual pathway refers to structures after this point.
Eye
editLight entering the eye isrefractedas it passes through thecornea.It then passes through thepupil(controlled by theiris) and is further refracted by thelens.The cornea and lens act together as a compound lens to project an inverted image onto the retina.
Retina
editThe retina consists of manyphotoreceptor cellswhich contain particularproteinmoleculescalledopsins.In humans, two types of opsins are involved in conscious vision:rod opsinsandcone opsins.(A third type,melanopsinin someretinal ganglion cells(RGC), part of thebody clockmechanism, is probably not involved in conscious vision, as these RGC do not project to thelateral geniculate nucleusbut to thepretectal olivary nucleus.[24]) An opsin absorbs aphoton(a particle of light) and transmits a signal to thecellthrough asignal transduction pathway,resulting in hyper-polarization of the photoreceptor.
Rods and cones differ in function. Rods are found primarily in the periphery of the retina and are used to see at low levels of light. Each human eye contains 120 million rods. Cones are found primarily in the center (orfovea) of the retina.[25]There are three types of cones that differ in thewavelengthsof light they absorb; they are usually called short or blue, middle or green, and long or red. Cones mediate day vision and can distinguishcolorand other features of the visual world at medium and high light levels. Cones are larger and much less numerous than rods (there are 6-7 million of them in each human eye).[25]
In the retina, the photoreceptorssynapsedirectly ontobipolar cells,which in turn synapse ontoganglion cellsof the outermost layer, which then conductaction potentialsto thebrain.A significant amount ofvisual processingarises from the patterns of communication betweenneuronsin the retina. About 130 million photo-receptors absorb light, yet roughly 1.2 millionaxonsof ganglion cells transmit information from the retina to the brain. The processing in the retina includes the formation of center-surroundreceptive fieldsof bipolar and ganglion cells in the retina, as well as convergence and divergence from photoreceptor to bipolar cell. In addition, other neurons in the retina, particularlyhorizontalandamacrine cells,transmit information laterally (from a neuron in one layer to an adjacent neuron in the same layer), resulting in more complex receptive fields that can be either indifferent to color and sensitive tomotionor sensitive to color and indifferent to motion.[26]
Mechanism of generating visual signals
editThe retina adapts to change in light through the use of the rods. In the dark, thechromophoreretinalhas a bent shape called cis-retinal (referring to acisconformation in one of the double bonds). When light interacts with the retinal, it changes conformation to a straight form called trans-retinal and breaks away from the opsin. This is called bleaching because the purifiedrhodopsinchanges from violet to colorless in the light. At baseline in the dark, the rhodopsin absorbs no light and releasesglutamate,which inhibits the bipolar cell. This inhibits the release of neurotransmitters from the bipolar cells to the ganglion cell. When there is light present, glutamate secretion ceases, thus no longer inhibiting the bipolar cell from releasing neurotransmitters to the ganglion cell and therefore an image can be detected.[27][28]
The final result of all this processing is five different populations of ganglion cells that send visual (image-forming and non-image-forming) information to the brain:[26]
- M cells, with large center-surround receptive fields that are sensitive todepth,indifferent to color, and rapidly adapt to a stimulus;
- P cells, with smaller center-surround receptive fields that are sensitive to color andshape;
- K cells, with very large center-only receptive fields that are sensitive to color and indifferent to shape or depth;
- another population that is intrinsically photosensitive;and
- a final population that is used for eye movements.[26]
A 2006University of Pennsylvaniastudy calculated the approximatebandwidthof human retinas to be about 8,960kilobitsper second, whereasguinea pigretinas transfer at about 875 kilobits.[29]
In 2007 Zaidi and co-researchers on both sides of the Atlantic studying patients without rods and cones, discovered that the novel photoreceptive ganglion cell in humans also has a role in conscious and unconscious visual perception.[30]The peakspectral sensitivitywas 481 nm. This shows that there are two pathways for vision in the retina – one based on classic photoreceptors (rods and cones) and the other, newly discovered, based on photo-receptive ganglion cells which act as rudimentary visual brightness detectors.
Photochemistry
editThe functioning of acamerais often compared with the workings of the eye, mostly since both focus light from external objects in thefield of viewonto a light-sensitive medium. In the case of the camera, this medium is film or an electronic sensor; in the case of the eye, it is an array of visual receptors. With this simple geometrical similarity, based on the laws of optics, the eye functions as atransducer,as does aCCD camera.
In the visual system,retinal,technically calledretinene1or "retinaldehyde", is a light-sensitive molecule found in the rods and cones of theretina.Retinal is the fundamental structure involved in the transduction oflightinto visual signals, i.e. nerve impulses in the ocular system of thecentral nervous system.In the presence of light, the retinal molecule changes configuration and as a result, anerve impulseis generated.[26]
Optic nerve
editThe information about the image via the eye is transmitted to the brain along theoptic nerve.Different populations of ganglion cells in the retina send information to the brain through the optic nerve. About 90% of theaxonsin the optic nerve go to thelateral geniculate nucleusin thethalamus.These axons originate from the M, P, and K ganglion cells in the retina, see above. Thisparallel processingis important for reconstructing the visual world; each type of information will go through a different route toperception.Another population sends information to thesuperior colliculusin themidbrain,which assists in controlling eye movements (saccades)[31]as well as other motor responses.
A final population ofphotosensitive ganglion cells,containingmelanopsinforphotosensitivity,sends information via theretinohypothalamic tractto thepretectum(pupillary reflex), to several structures involved in the control ofcircadian rhythmsandsleepsuch as thesuprachiasmatic nucleus(the biological clock), and to theventrolateral preoptic nucleus(a region involved insleep regulation).[32]A recently discovered role for photoreceptive ganglion cells is that they mediate conscious and unconscious vision – acting as rudimentary visual brightness detectors as shown in rodless coneless eyes.[30]
Optic chiasm
editThe optic nerves from both eyes meet and cross at the optic chiasm,[33][34]at the base of thehypothalamusof the brain. At this point, the information coming from both eyes is combined and then splits according to thevisual field.The corresponding halves of the field of view (right and left) are sent to the left and righthalves of the brain,respectively, to be processed. That is, the right side ofprimary visual cortexdeals with the left half of thefield of viewfrom both eyes, and similarly for the left brain.[31]A small region in the center of the field of view is processed redundantly by both halves of the brain.
Optic tract
editInformation from the rightvisual field(now on the left side of the brain) travels in the left optic tract. Information from the leftvisual fieldtravels in the right optic tract. Each optic tract terminates in thelateral geniculate nucleus(LGN) in the thalamus.
Lateral geniculate nucleus
editThelateral geniculate nucleus(LGN) is a sensory relay nucleus in the thalamus of the brain. The LGN consists of six layers inhumansand otherprimatesstarting fromcatarrhines,includingcercopithecidaeandapes.Layers 1, 4, and 6 correspond to information from the contralateral (crossed) fibers of the nasal retina (temporal visual field); layers 2, 3, and 5 correspond toinformationfrom the ipsilateral (uncrossed) fibers of the temporal retina (nasal visual field).
Layer one contains M cells, which correspond to the M (magnocellular) cells of the optic nerve of the opposite eye and are concerned with depth or motion. Layers four and six of the LGN also connect to the opposite eye, but to the P cells (color and edges) of the optic nerve. By contrast, layers two, three and five of the LGN connect to the M cells and P (parvocellular) cells of the optic nerve for the same side of the brain as its respective LGN.
Spread out, the six layers of the LGN are the area of acredit cardand about three times its thickness. The LGN is rolled up into twoellipsoidsabout the size and shape of two small birds' eggs. In between the six layers are smaller cells that receive information from the K cells (color) in the retina. The neurons of the LGN then relay the visual image to theprimary visual cortex(V1) which is located at the back of the brain (posterior end) in theoccipital lobein and close to thecalcarine sulcus.The LGN is not just a simple relay station, but it is also a center for processing; it receives reciprocal input from thecorticaland subcortical layers andreciprocal innervationfrom the visual cortex.[26]
Optic radiation
editTheoptic radiations,one on each side of the brain, carry information from the thalamiclateral geniculate nucleusto layer 4 of thevisual cortex.The P layer neurons of the LGN relay to V1 layer 4C β. The M layer neurons relay to V1 layer 4C α. The K layer neurons in the LGN relay to large neurons called blobs in layers 2 and 3 of V1.[26]
There is a direct correspondence from an angular position in thevisual fieldof the eye, all the way through the optic tract to a nerve position in V1 up to V4, i.e. the primary visual areas. After that, the visual pathway is roughly separated into aventral and dorsal pathway.
Visual cortex
editThe visual cortex is responsible for processing the visual image. It lies at the rear of the brain (highlighted in the image), above thecerebellum.The region that receives information directly from the LGN is called theprimary visual cortex(also called V1 and striate cortex). It creates a bottom-up saliency map of the visual field to guide attention or eye gaze to salient visual locations.[35][clarification needed]Hence selection of visual input information by attention starts at V1[36]along the visual pathway.
Visual information then flows through a cortical hierarchy. These areas include V2, V3, V4 and area V5/MT. (The exact connectivity depends on the species of the animal.) These secondary visual areas (collectively termed the extrastriate visual cortex) process a wide variety of visual primitives. Neurons in V1 and V2 respond selectively to bars of specific orientations, or combinations of bars. These are believed to support edge and corner detection. Similarly, basic information about color and motion is processed here.[37]
Heider, et al. (2002) found that neurons involving V1, V2, and V3 can detect stereoscopicillusory contours;they found that stereoscopic stimuli subtending up to 8° can activate these neurons.[38]
Visual association cortex
editAs visual information passes forward through the visual hierarchy, the complexity of the neural representations increases. Whereas a V1 neuron may respond selectively to a line segment of a particular orientation in a particularretinotopiclocation, neurons in the lateral occipital complex respond selectively to a complete object (e.g., a figure drawing), and neurons in the visual association cortex may respond selectively to human faces, or to a particular object.
Along with this increasing complexity of neural representation may come a level of specialization of processing into two distinct pathways: thedorsal streamand theventral stream(theTwo Streams hypothesis,[39]first proposed by Ungerleider and Mishkin in 1982). The dorsal stream, commonly referred to as the "where" stream, is involved in spatial attention (covert and overt), and communicates with regions that control eye movements and hand movements. More recently, this area has been called the "how" stream to emphasize its role in guiding behaviors to spatial locations. The ventral stream, commonly referred to as the "what" stream, is involved in the recognition, identification and categorization of visual stimuli.
However, there is still much debate about the degree of specialization within these two pathways, since they are in fact heavily interconnected.[40]
Horace Barlowproposed theefficient coding hypothesisin 1961 as a theoretical model ofsensory codingin thebrain.[41]Limitations in the applicability of this theory in theprimary visual cortex (V1)motivated theV1 Saliency Hypothesisthat V1 creates a bottom-up saliency map to guide attention exogenously.[35]With attentional selection as a center stage, vision is seen as composed of encoding, selection, and decoding stages.[42]
Thedefault mode networkis a network of brain regions that are active when an individual is awake and at rest. The visual system's default mode can be monitored duringresting state fMRI: Fox, et al. (2005) found that "the human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks ",[43]in which the visual system switches from resting state to attention.
In theparietal lobe,thelateraland ventral intraparietal cortex are involved in visual attention and saccadic eye movements. These regions are in theintraparietal sulcus(marked in red in the adjacent image).
Development
editInfancy
editNewborn infants have limitedcolor perception.[44]One study found that 74% of newborns can distinguish red, 36% green, 25% yellow, and 14% blue. After one month, performance "improved somewhat."[45]Infant's eyes do not have the ability toaccommodate.Pediatricians are able to perform non-verbal testing to assessvisual acuityof a newborn, detectnearsightednessandastigmatism,and evaluate the eye teaming and alignment. Visual acuity improves from about 20/400 at birth to approximately 20/25 at 6 months of age. This happens because the nerve cells in theretinaand brain that control vision are not fully developed.
Childhood and adolescence
editDepth perception,focus, tracking and other aspects of vision continue to develop throughout early and middle childhood. From recent studies in theUnited StatesandAustraliathere is some evidence that the amount of time school aged children spend outdoors, in natural light, may have some impact on whether they developmyopia.The condition tends to get somewhat worse through childhood and adolescence, but stabilizes in adulthood. More prominent myopia (nearsightedness) and astigmatism are thought to be inherited. Children with this condition may need to wear glasses.
Adulthood
editVision is often one of the first senses affected by aging. A number of changes occur with aging:
- Over time, thelensbecomes yellowed and may eventually become brown, a condition known as brunescence orbrunescentcataract.Although many factors contribute to yellowing, lifetime exposure toultraviolet lightandagingare two main causes.
- The lens becomes less flexible, diminishing the ability to accommodate (presbyopia).
- While a healthy adult pupil typically has a size range of 2–8 mm, with age the range gets smaller, trending towards a moderately small diameter.
- On averagetear productiondeclines with age. However, there are a number of age-related conditions that can cause excessive tearing.
Other functions
editBalance
editAlong withproprioceptionandvestibular function,the visual system plays an important role in the ability of an individual to control balance and maintain an upright posture. When these three conditions are isolated and balance is tested, it has been found that vision is the most significant contributor to balance, playing a bigger role than either of the two other intrinsic mechanisms.[46]The clarity with which an individual can see his environment, as well as the size of the visual field, the susceptibility of the individual to light and glare, and poor depth perception play important roles in providing a feedback loop to the brain on the body's movement through the environment. Anything that affects any of these variables can have a negative effect on balance and maintaining posture.[47]This effect has been seen in research involving elderly subjects when compared to young controls,[48]inglaucomapatients compared to age matched controls,[49]cataractpatients pre and post surgery,[50]and even something as simple as wearing safety goggles.[51]Monocular vision(one eyed vision) has also been shown to negatively impact balance, which was seen in the previously referenced cataract and glaucoma studies,[49][50]as well as in healthy children and adults.[52]
According to Pollock et al. (2010)strokeis the main cause of specific visual impairment, most frequently visual field loss (homonymous hemianopia,a visual field defect). Nevertheless, evidence for the efficacy of cost-effective interventions aimed at these visual field defects is still inconsistent.[53]
Clinical significance
editProper function of the visual system is required for sensing, processing, and understanding the surrounding environment. Difficulty in sensing, processing and understanding light input has the potential to adversely impact an individual's ability to communicate, learn and effectively complete routine tasks on a daily basis.
In children, early diagnosis and treatment of impaired visual system function is an important factor in ensuring that key social, academic and speech/language developmental milestones are met.
Cataractis clouding of the lens, which in turn affects vision. Although it may be accompanied by yellowing, clouding and yellowing can occur separately. This is typically a result of ageing, disease, or drug use.
Presbyopiais a visual condition that causesfarsightedness.The eye's lens becomes too inflexible toaccommodateto normal reading distance, focus tending to remain fixed at long distance.
Glaucomais a type of blindness that begins at the edge of the visual field and progresses inward. It may result intunnel vision.This typically involves the outer layers of the optic nerve, sometimes as a result of buildup of fluid and excessive pressure in the eye.[54]
Scotomais a type of blindness that produces a smallblind spotin the visual field typically caused by injury in the primary visual cortex.
Homonymous hemianopiais a type of blindness that destroys one entire side of the visual field typically caused by injury in the primary visual cortex.
Quadrantanopiais a type of blindness that destroys only a part of the visual field typically caused by partial injury in the primary visual cortex. This is very similar to homonymous hemianopia, but to a lesser degree.
Prosopagnosia,or face blindness, is a brain disorder that produces an inability to recognize faces. This disorder often arises after damage to thefusiform face area.
Visual agnosia,or visual-form agnosia, is a brain disorder that produces an inability to recognize objects. This disorder often arises after damage to theventral stream.
Other animals
editDifferentspeciesare able to see different parts of thelight spectrum;for example,beescan see into theultraviolet,[55]whilepit viperscan accurately target prey with theirpit organs,which are sensitive to infrared radiation.[56]Themantis shrimppossesses arguably the most complex visual system of any species. The eye of the mantis shrimp holds 16 color receptive cones, whereas humans only have three. The variety of cones enables them to perceive an enhanced array of colors as a mechanism for mate selection, avoidance of predators, and detection of prey.[57]Swordfish also possess an impressive visual system. The eye of aswordfishcan generateheatto better cope with detecting theirpreyat depths of 2000 feet.[58]Certainone-celledmicroorganisms,thewarnowiiddinoflagellateshave eye-likeocelloids,with analogous structures for the lens and retina of the multi-cellular eye.[59]The armored shell of thechitonAcanthopleura granulatais also covered with hundreds ofaragonitecrystalline eyes, namedocelli,which can formimages.[60]
Manyfan worms,such asAcromegalomma interruptumwhich live in tubes on the sea floor of theGreat Barrier Reef,have evolved compound eyes on their tentacles, which they use to detect encroaching movement. If movement is detected, the fan worms will rapidly withdraw their tentacles. Bok, et al., have discovered opsins andG proteinsin the fan worm's eyes, which were previously only seen in simpleciliaryphotoreceptors in the brains of someinvertebrates,as opposed to therhabdomericreceptors in the eyes of most invertebrates.[61]
Onlyhigher primateOld World(African)monkeysand apes (macaques,apes,orangutans) have the same kind of three-conephotoreceptorcolor vision humans have, while lower primateNew World(South American) monkeys (spider monkeys,squirrel monkeys,cebus monkeys) have a two-cone photoreceptor kind of color vision.[62]
Biologists have determined that humans have extremely good vision compared to the overwhelming majority of animals, particularly in daylight, though a few species have better.[63]Other animals such asdogsare thought to rely more on senses other than vision, which in turn may be better developed than in humans.[64][65]
History
editIn the second half of the 19th century, many motifs of the nervous system were identified such as the neuron doctrine and brain localization, which related to theneuronbeing the basic unit of the nervous system and functional localisation in the brain, respectively. These would become tenets of the fledglingneuroscienceand would support further understanding of the visual system.
The notion that thecerebral cortexis divided into functionally distinct cortices now known to be responsible for capacities such astouch(somatosensory cortex),movement(motor cortex), and vision (visual cortex), was first proposed byFranz Joseph Gallin 1810.[66]Evidence for functionally distinct areas of the brain (and, specifically, of the cerebral cortex) mounted throughout the 19th century with discoveries byPaul Brocaof thelanguage center(1861), andGustav FritschandEduard Hitzigof the motor cortex (1871).[66][67]Based on selective damage to parts of the brain and the functional effects of the resultinglesions,David Ferrierproposed that visual function was localized to theparietal lobeof the brain in 1876.[67]In 1881,Hermann Munkmore accurately located vision in theoccipital lobe,where theprimary visual cortexis now known to be.[67]
In 2014, a textbook "Understanding vision: theory, models, and data"[42]illustrates how to link neurobiological data and visual behavior/psychological data through theoretical principles and computational models.
See also
edit- Achromatopsia
- Akinetopsia
- Apperceptive agnosia
- Associative visual agnosia
- Asthenopia
- Astigmatism
- Color blindness
- Echolocation
- Computer vision
- Helmholtz–Kohlrausch effect– howcolor balanceaffects vision
- Magnocellular cell
- Memory-prediction framework
- Prosopagnosia
- Scotopic sensitivity syndrome
- Recovery from blindness
- Visual agnosia
- Visual modularity
- Visual perception
- Visual processing
References
edit- ^"How the Human Eye Sees."WebMD.Ed. Alan Kozarsky. WebMD, 3 October 2015. Web. 27 March 2016.
- ^Than, Ker. "How the Human Eye Works."LiveScience.TechMedia Network,10 February 2010. Web. 27 March 2016.
- ^"How the Human Eye Works | Cornea Layers/Role | Light Rays." NKCF. The Gavin Herbert Eye Institute. Web. 27 March 2016.
- ^Albertine, Kurt. Barron's Anatomy Flash Cards
- ^Tillotson, Joanne. McCann, Stephanie. Kaplan's Medical Flashcards. April 2, 2013.
- ^"Optic Chiasma." Optic Chiasm Function, Anatomy & Definition. Healthline Medical Team, 9 March 2015. Web. 27 March 2016.
- ^Jefferey, G., and M. M. Neveu. "Chiasm Formation in Man Is Fundamentally Different from That in the Mouse."Nature.com.Nature Publishing Group,21 March 2007. Web. 27 March 2016.
- ^Card, J. Patrick, and Robert Y. Moore. "Organization of Lateral Geniculate-hypothalamic Connections in the Rat."Wiley Online Library.1 June. 1989. Web. 27 March 2016.
- ^Murphy, Penelope C.; Duckett, Simon G.; Sillito, Adam M. (1999-11-19)."Feedback Connections to the Lateral Geniculate Nucleus and Cortical Response Properties".Science.286(5444): 1552–1554.doi:10.1126/science.286.5444.1552.ISSN0036-8075.PMID10567260.
- ^Schiller, P. H.; Malpeli, J. G. (1978-05-01)."Functional specificity of lateral geniculate nucleus laminae of the rhesus monkey".Journal of Neurophysiology.41(3): 788–797.doi:10.1152/jn.1978.41.3.788.ISSN0022-3077.PMID96227.
- ^Schmielau, F.; Singer, W. (1977)."The role of visual cortex for binocular interactions in the cat lateral geniculate nucleus".Brain Research.120(2): 354–361.doi:10.1016/0006-8993(77)90914-3.PMID832128.S2CID28796357.
- ^Clay Reid, R.; Alonso, Jose-Manuel (1995-11-16)."Specificity of monosynaptic connections from thalamus to visual cortex".Nature.378(6554): 281–284.Bibcode:1995Natur.378..281C.doi:10.1038/378281a0.ISSN0028-0836.PMID7477347.S2CID4285683.
- ^Zhaoping, Li (2014-05-08). "The V1 hypothesis—creating a bottom-up saliency map for preattentive selection and segmentation".Understanding Vision: Theory, Models, and Data(1st ed.). Oxford University Press.doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199564668.001.0001.ISBN978-0-19-956466-8.
- ^Heim, Stefan; Eickhoff, Simon B.; Ischebeck, Anja K.; Friederici, Angela D.; Stephan, Klaas E.; Amunts, Katrin (2009)."Effective connectivity of the left BA 44, BA 45, and inferior temporal gyrus during lexical and phonological decisions identified with DCM".Human Brain Mapping.30(2): 392–402.doi:10.1002/hbm.20512.ISSN1065-9471.PMC6870893.PMID18095285.
- ^Catani, Marco, and Derek K. Jones. "Brain." Occipito‐temporal Connections in the Human Brain. 23 June 2003. Web. 27 March 2016.
- ^Benevento, Louis A.; Standage, Gregg P. (1983-07-01)."The organization of projections of the retinorecipient and nonretinorecipient nuclei of the pretectal complex and layers of the superior colliculus to the lateral pulvinar and medial pulvinar in the macaque monkey".Journal of Comparative Neurology.217(3): 307–336.doi:10.1002/cne.902170307.ISSN0021-9967.PMID6886056.S2CID44794002.
- ^Hirsch, Ja; Gilbert, Cd (1991-06-01)."Synaptic physiology of horizontal connections in the cat's visual cortex".The Journal of Neuroscience.11(6): 1800–1809.doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-06-01800.1991.ISSN0270-6474.PMC6575415.PMID1675266.
- ^Schall, JD; Morel, A.; King, DJ; Bullier, J. (1995-06-01)."Topography of visual cortex connections with frontal eye field in macaque: convergence and segregation of processing streams".The Journal of Neuroscience.15(6): 4464–4487.doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-06-04464.1995.ISSN0270-6474.PMC6577698.PMID7540675.
- ^Moser, May-Britt, and Edvard I. Moser. "Functional Differentiation in the Hippocampus." Wiley Online Library. 1998. Web. 27 March 2016.
- ^Kanaseki, T.; Sprague, J. M. (1974-12-01)."Anatomical organization of pretectal nuclei and tectal laminae in the cat".Journal of Comparative Neurology.158(3): 319–337.doi:10.1002/cne.901580307.ISSN0021-9967.PMID4436458.S2CID38463227.
- ^Reiner, Anton, and Harvey J. Karten. "Parasympathetic Ocular Control — Functional Subdivisions and Circuitry of the Avian Nucleus of Edinger-Westphal." Science Direct. 1983. Web. 27 March 2016.
- ^Welsh, David K; Logothetis, Diomedes E; Meister, Markus; Reppert, Steven M (April 1995)."Individual neurons dissociated from rat suprachiasmatic nucleus express independently phased circadian firing rhythms".Neuron.14(4): 697–706.doi:10.1016/0896-6273(95)90214-7.PMID7718233.
- ^"The Optic Pathway - Eye Disorders".MSD Manual Professional Edition.Retrieved18 January2022.
- ^Güler, A.D.; et al. (May 2008)."Melanopsin cells are the principal conduits for rod/cone input to non-image forming vision"(Abstract).Nature.453(7191): 102–5.Bibcode:2008Natur.453..102G.doi:10.1038/nature06829.PMC2871301.PMID18432195.
- ^abNave, R."Light and Vision".HyperPhysics.Retrieved2014-11-13.
- ^abcdefTovée 2008
- ^Saladin, Kenneth D.Anatomy & Physiology: The Unity of Form and Function.5th ed. New York:McGraw-Hill,2010.
- ^"Webvision: Ganglion cell Physiology".Archived fromthe originalon 2011-01-23.Retrieved2018-12-08.
- ^"Calculating the speed of sight".
- ^abZaidi FH, Hull JT, Peirson SN, et al. (December 2007)."Short-wavelength light sensitivity of circadian, pupillary, and visual awareness in humans lacking an outer retina".Curr. Biol.17(24): 2122–8.Bibcode:2007CBio...17.2122Z.doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.11.034.PMC2151130.PMID18082405.
- ^abSundsten, John W.; Nolte, John (2001).The human brain: an introduction to its functional anatomy.St. Louis: Mosby. pp. 410–447.ISBN978-0-323-01320-8.OCLC47892833.
- ^Lucas RJ, Hattar S, Takao M, Berson DM, Foster RG, Yau KW (January 2003). "Diminished pupillary light reflex at high irradiances in melanopsin-knockout mice".Science.299(5604): 245–7.Bibcode:2003Sci...299..245L.CiteSeerX10.1.1.1028.8525.doi:10.1126/science.1077293.PMID12522249.S2CID46505800.
- ^Turner, Howard R. (1997)."Optics".Science in medieval Islam: an illustrated introduction.Austin: University of Texas Press. p.197.ISBN978-0-292-78149-8.OCLC440896281.
- ^Vesalius 1543
- ^abLi, Z (2002)."A saliency map in primary visual cortex".Trends in Cognitive Sciences.6(1): 9–16.doi:10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01817-9.PMID11849610.S2CID13411369.
- ^Zhaoping, L. (2019)."A new framework for understanding vision from the perspective of the primary visual cortex".Current Opinion in Neurobiology.58:1–10.doi:10.1016/j.conb.2019.06.001.PMID31271931.S2CID195806018.
- ^Jessell, Thomas M.; Kandel, Eric R.; Schwartz, James H. (2000). "27. Central visual pathways".Principles of neural science.New York: McGraw-Hill. pp.533–540.ISBN978-0-8385-7701-1.OCLC42073108.
- ^Heider, Barbara; Spillmann, Lothar; Peterhans, Esther (2002) "Stereoscopic Illusory Contours— Cortical Neuron Responses and Human Perception"J. Cognitive Neuroscience14:7 pp.1018-29Archived2016-10-11 at theWayback Machineaccessdate=2014-05-18
- ^Mishkin M, Ungerleider LG (1982). "Contribution of striate inputs to the visuospatial functions of parieto-preoccipital cortex in monkeys".Behav. Brain Res.6(1): 57–77.doi:10.1016/0166-4328(82)90081-X.PMID7126325.S2CID33359587.
- ^Farivar R. (2009). "Dorsal-ventral integration in object recognition".Brain Res. Rev.61(2): 144–53.doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev.2009.05.006.PMID19481571.S2CID6817815.
- ^Barlow, H. (1961) "Possible principles underlying the transformation of sensory messages" inSensory Communication,MIT Press
- ^abZhaoping, Li (2014).Understanding vision: theory, models, and data.United Kingdom: Oxford University Press.ISBN978-0-19-882936-2.
- ^Fox, Michael D.; et al. (2005)."From The Cover: The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks".PNAS.102(27): 9673–9678.Bibcode:2005PNAS..102.9673F.doi:10.1073/pnas.0504136102.PMC1157105.PMID15976020.
- ^Lane, Kenneth A. (2012).Visual Attention in Children: Theories and Activities.SLACK. p. 7.ISBN978-1-55642-956-9.Retrieved4 December2014.
- ^Adams, Russell J.; Courage, Mary L.; Mercer, Michele E. (1994). "Systematic measurement of human neonatal color vision".Vision Research.34(13): 1691–1701.doi:10.1016/0042-6989(94)90127-9.ISSN0042-6989.PMID7941376.S2CID27842977.
- ^Hansson EE, Beckman A, Håkansson A (December 2010)."Effect of vision, proprioception, and the position of the vestibular organ on postural sway"(PDF).Acta Otolaryngol.130(12): 1358–63.doi:10.3109/00016489.2010.498024.PMID20632903.S2CID36949084.
- ^Wade MG, Jones G (June 1997)."The role of vision and spatial orientation in the maintenance of posture".Phys Ther.77(6): 619–28.doi:10.1093/ptj/77.6.619.PMID9184687.
- ^Teasdale N, Stelmach GE, Breunig A (November 1991). "Postural sway characteristics of the elderly under normal and altered visual and support surface conditions".J Gerontol.46(6): B238–44.doi:10.1093/geronj/46.6.B238.PMID1940075.
- ^abShabana N, Cornilleau-Pérès V, Droulez J, Goh JC, Lee GS, Chew PT (June 2005). "Postural stability in primary open angle glaucoma".Clin. Experiment. Ophthalmol.33(3): 264–73.doi:10.1111/j.1442-9071.2005.01003.x.PMID15932530.S2CID26286705.
- ^abSchwartz S, Segal O, Barkana Y, Schwesig R, Avni I, Morad Y (March 2005)."The effect of cataract surgery on postural control".Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci.46(3): 920–4.doi:10.1167/iovs.04-0543.PMID15728548.
- ^Wade LR, Weimar WH, Davis J (December 2004). "Effect of personal protective eyewear on postural stability".Ergonomics.47(15): 1614–23.doi:10.1080/00140130410001724246.PMID15545235.S2CID22219417.
- ^Barela JA, Sanches M, Lopes AG, Razuk M, Moraes R (2011)."Use of monocular and binocular visual cues for postural control in children".J Vis.11(12): 10.doi:10.1167/11.12.10.PMID22004694.
- ^"Vision".International Journal of Stroke.5(3_suppl): 67. 2010.doi:10.1111/j.1747-4949.2010.00516.x.
- ^Harvard Health Publications (2010).The Aging Eye: Preventing and treating eye disease.Harvard Health Publications. p. 20.ISBN978-1-935555-16-2.Retrieved15 December2014.
- ^Bellingham J, Wilkie SE, Morris AG, Bowmaker JK, Hunt DM (February 1997)."Characterisation of the ultraviolet-sensitive opsin gene in the honey bee, Apis mellifera".Eur. J. Biochem.243(3): 775–81.doi:10.1111/j.1432-1033.1997.00775.x.PMID9057845.
- ^Safer AB, Grace MS (September 2004). "Infrared imaging in vipers: differential responses of crotaline and viperine snakes to paired thermal targets".Behav. Brain Res.154(1): 55–61.doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2004.01.020.PMID15302110.S2CID39736880.
- ^"(2018)" Peacock Mantis Shrimp "National Aquarium".Archived fromthe originalon 2018-05-04.Retrieved2018-03-06.
- ^David Fleshler(10-15-2012)South Florida Sun-SentinelArchived2013-02-03 atarchive.today,
- ^Single-Celled Planktonic Organisms Have Animal-Like Eyes, Scientists Say
- ^Li, L; Connors, MJ; Kolle, M; England, GT; Speiser, DI; Xiao, X; Aizenberg, J; Ortiz, C (2015)."Multifunctionality of chiton biomineralized armor with an integrated visual system".Science.350(6263): 952–6.doi:10.1126/science.aad1246.hdl:1721.1/100035.PMID26586760.
- ^Bok, Michael J.; Porter, Megan L.; Nilsson, Dan-Eric (July 2017)."Phototransduction in fan worm radiolar eyes".Current Biology.27(14): R698–R699.Bibcode:2017CBio...27.R698B.doi:10.1016/j.cub.2017.05.093.hdl:1983/3793ef99-753c-4c60-8d91-92815395387a.PMID28743013.cited byEvolution of fan worm eyes (August 1, 2017) Phys.org
- ^Margaret., Livingstone (2008).Vision and art: the biology of seeing.Hubel, David H. New York: Abrams.ISBN978-0-8109-9554-3.OCLC192082768.
- ^Renner, Ben (January 9, 2019)."Which species, including humans, has the sharpest vision? Study debunks old beliefs".Study Finds.RetrievedFebruary 25,2024.
- ^Gibeault, Stephanie (March 22, 2018)."Do Dogs Have Self-Awareness?".American Kennel Club.RetrievedFebruary 25,2024.
- ^"Animal senses: How they differ from humans".Animalpha.September 14, 2023.RetrievedFebruary 25,2024.
- ^abGross CG (1994). "How inferior temporal cortex became a visual area".Cereb. Cortex.4(5): 455–69.doi:10.1093/cercor/4.5.455.PMID7833649.
- ^abcSchiller PH (1986). "The central visual system".Vision Res.26(9): 1351–86.doi:10.1016/0042-6989(86)90162-8.ISSN0042-6989.PMID3303663.S2CID5247746.
Further reading
edit- Davison JA, Patel AS, Cunha JP, Schwiegerling J, Muftuoglu O (July 2011)."Recent studies provide an updated clinical perspective on blue light-filtering IOLs".Graefes Arch. Clin. Exp. Ophthalmol.249(7): 957–68.doi:10.1007/s00417-011-1697-6.PMC3124647.PMID21584764.
- Hatori M, Panda S (October 2010)."The emerging roles of melanopsin in behavioral adaptation to light".Trends Mol Med.16(10): 435–46.doi:10.1016/j.molmed.2010.07.005.PMC2952704.PMID20810319.
- Heiting, G., (2011). Your infant's vision Development. Retrieved February 27, 2012 fromhttp://www.allaboutvision.com/parents/infants.htm
- Hubel, David H. (1995).Eye, brain, and vision.New York:Scientific American Library.ISBN978-0-7167-6009-2.OCLC32806252.
- Kolb B, Whishaw I (2012).Introduction to Brain and Behaviour Fourth Edition.New York: Worth Publishers.ISBN978-1-4292-4228-8.OCLC918592547.
- Marr, David; Ullman, Shimon; Poggio, Tomaso (2010).Vision: A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information.Cambridge, Mass:The MIT Press.ISBN978-0-262-51462-0.OCLC472791457.
- Rodiek, R.W. (1988). "The Primate Retina".Comparative Primate Biology.Neurosciences.4.New York: A.R. Liss..(H.D. Steklis and J. Erwin, editors.) pp. 203–278.
- Schmolesky, Matthew (1995)."The Primary Visual Cortex".NIH National Library of Medicine.PMID21413385.
- The Aging Eye; See into Your future. (2009). Retrieved February 27, 2012 fromhttps://web.archive.org/web/20111117045917/http://www.realage.com/check-your-health/eye-health/aging-eye
- Tovée, Martin J. (2008).An introduction to the visual system.Cambridge, UK:Cambridge University Press.ISBN978-0-521-88319-1.OCLC185026571.
- Vesalius, Andreas(1543).De Humani Corporis Fabrica[On the Workings of the Human Body].
- Wiesel, Torsten;Hubel, David H.(1963). "The effects of visual deprivation on the morphology and physiology of cell's lateral geniculate body".Journal of Neurophysiology.26(6): 978–993.doi:10.1152/jn.1963.26.6.978.PMID14084170.S2CID16117515..
External links
edit- "Webvision: The Organization of the Retina and Visual System"– John Moran Eye Center at University of Utah
- VisionScience.com– An online resource for researchers in vision science.
- Journal of Vision– An online, open access journal of vision science.
- i-Perception– An online, open access journal of perception science.
- Hagfish research has found the "missing link" in the evolution of the eye. See:Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
- Valentin Dragoi."Chapter 14: Visual Processing: Eye and Retina".Neuroscience Online, the Open-Access Neuroscience Electronic Textbook.The University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston (UTHealth). Archived fromthe originalon 1 November 2017.Retrieved27 April2014.