In colloquial usage,contemptusually refers to either the act of despising, or having a general lack ofrespectfor something. This set of emotions generally produces maladaptive behaviour.[1][2]Other authors define contempt as anegative emotionrather than the constellation of mentality and feelings that produce anattitude.Paul Ekmancategorises contempt as the seventhbasic emotion,along withanger,disgust,fear,happiness,sadnessandsurprise.Robert C. Solomonplaces contempt on the same emotional continuum asresentmentand anger, and he argues that the differences between the three are thatresentmentis anger directed towards a higher-status individual;angeris directed towards an equal-status individual; and contempt is anger directed towards a lower-status individual.[3][page needed]

This picture of Thomas Ward, arrested for stealing a £1 coin, can be seen as showing contempt.
A painting byLouis-Léopold Boilly(ca. 1797).
The woman has been interpreted as aprostitute(who isdisdainingthe inadequate coin proffered by the fashionable gentleman getting his shoes shined at left).

Etymology

edit

The term originated in 1393[citation needed]in Old French from theLatinwordcontemptusmeaning "scorn". It is thepast participleofcontemnereand fromcon-intensiveprefix+temnere"to slight, scorn";contemptuousappeared in 1529.[4]

Cultural context

edit
Facial expression of contempt

EkmanandFriesen(1986) identified a specificfacial expressionthat observers in ten different cultures, both Western and non-Western, agreed signaled contempt. In this study, residents ofWest Sumatra,Indonesia, viewed photos of American, Japanese, and Indonesian people. Their ability to classify some facial expressions as contempt versus the primary emotions of anger, disgust, happiness, sadness, fear, or surprise showed that across cultures, general contempt is universally understood (with level of agreement equating to 75%).[5] "An expression in which the corner of the lip is tightened and raised slightly on one side of the face (or much more strongly on one side than the other) signaled contempt." This study showed that contempt, as well as the outward expression of contempt, can be pointed out across Western and Non-Western peoples when contrasted with other primary emotions.

InAmerican Englishthe use of the word "contempt" has declined since the early 19th century, while in the 21st century the word "disrespect"has become relatively more common.[6]

Characteristics

edit

Paul Ekman,a widely recognized psychologist, found six emotions that were universally recognized: anger, disgust, fear, joy, sadness, and surprise. Findings on contempt are less clear, though there is at least some preliminary evidence that this emotion and its expression are universally recognized.[7]

In the 1990s, Ekman proposed an expanded list of emotions, this time including contempt.[8]

Defining features

edit

Contempt has five features.[9]Contempt requires a judgment concerning the appearance or standing of the object of contempt. In particular, contempt involves the judgment that, because of some moral or personal failing or defect, the contemned person has compromised his or her standingvis-à-visan interpersonal standard that the contemptor treats as important. This may have not been done deliberately but by a lack ofstatus.This lack of status may cause the contemptuous to classify the object of contempt as utterly worthless, or as not fully meeting a particular interpersonal standard. Therefore, contempt is a response to a perceived failure to meet an interpersonal standard. Contempt is also a particular way of regarding or attending to the object of contempt, and this form of regard has an unpleasantaffectiveelement. Contempt may be experienced as a highly visceral emotion similar to disgust, or as cool disregard.

Facial expression showing subtle contempt

Contempt has a certain comparative element. InDavid Hume's studies of contempt, he suggests that contempt essentially requires apprehending the "bad qualities" of someone "as they really are" while simultaneously making a comparison between this person and ourselves. Because of this reflexive element, contempt also involves what we might term a "positive self-feeling" of the contemptuous. A characteristic of contempt is the psychological withdrawal or distance one typically feels regarding the object of one's contempt. This psychological distancing is an essential way of expressing one's nonidentification with the object of one's contempt and it precludes sympathetic identification with the object of contempt. (Hume, 2002, 251) Contempt for a person involves a way of negatively and comparatively regarding or attending to someone who has not fully lived up to an interpersonal standard that the person extending contempt thinks is important. This form of regard constitutes a psychological withdrawal from the object of contempt.[9]

Virtues

edit

Contempt can serve a useful purpose in a moral community. An ethics of contempt provides a much larger breadth of answers than other competing systems of ethics, whether they be based on ethics of actions (judging actions by their rightness or wrongness) or ethics of feelings (e.g., ethics of resentment). By feeling contempt for those things which are found to be unethical, immoral, or morally unsavory, one can both show that they are bad and remove them from the moral community.[10][page needed]

Response

edit

The main response of contempt lies within "publicized expression of low regard for the objects held in contempt" (Miller, C.H., 2005). By this reasoning, a person holding contempt would not have the urge to openly confront the person with whom they are at odds, nor would they themselves try to remove the object of contempt; rather, one who holds contempt would have the tendency to hold the view that others should remove the object of contempt, or hold the view that the object of contempt should remove itself. So while one would make their feelings known to others, the person with contempt would not necessarily want to directly deal with the situation at hand. One who is experiencing contempt would exhibit negative affective behaviors that may be labeled as "cold" – this simply meaning that one who is experiencing the emotion of contempt would tend toalienatethose responsible.[11][page needed]

Contempt in relationships

edit

Gender differences

edit

Men and women act differently when displaying contempt in same-sex relationships. Not only do girls engage in more non-verbal forms of social aggression than boys do, girls dissembled more than boys do, speaking nicely but making mean faces. In the research provided byUnderwood(2004) in their laboratory observation studies where they watch girls and boys in an identical social context in which best friends respond to a provoking newcomer, gender differences emerge not for the verbal behaviours, but for the nonverbal expressions of disdain and contempt (which are so glaring that they were observed with high degrees of inter-coder reliability by both women and men, kappa's exceeding.8; Underwood et al., 2003).[12]

There are several reasons why girls can be especially prone to conveying anger and contempt by nonverbal forms ofsocial exclusion.One reason may be that girls are socialized from infancy onward to be overtly nice and conciliatory and do so to avoid conflict whenever possible, for fear of beingexcluded from relationships,disliked, or punished (for reviews, see Brown and Gilligan, 1993; Underwood, 2003; Zahn-Waxler, 2000). Non-verbal forms of social exclusion may be a highly effective way to harm someone with relatively few social consequences; the hurtful act is fleeting, can often be executed behind the victim's back and outside of the watchful eyes of adults, and, even if caught, mean faces are typically not punished. Second, girls may hurt one another via non-verbal expressions of exclusion or disdain because girls and women may gaze at others more for reasons related to their lower social status, so as to learn as much as possible about others' needs and desires (see LaFrance, 2002, for a discussion of 'Smile boycotts and other body politics', p. 319).

Because girls and women gaze at others often, perhaps mean glares are more effective as a means of wielding power. Third, non-verbal forms of social exclusion may be powerful for girls because their relationships involve high levels of intimacy and self-disclosure (see Buhrmester and Prager, 1995, for a review), thus even subtle indicators of exclusion are threatening. Fourth, non-verbal forms of social exclusion may be powerful for girls because although they fiercely desire and defend popularity with other girls, they dread being labelled as 'stuck up' (Merten, 1997).[12]

edit

In 2003, thePalo AltoCity Council defeated a resolution that would have discouraged elected officials from facial expressions conveying contempt at public meetings; this was proposed because council members were so weary of colleagues intimidating one another by these subtle but rude facial expressions.[13]

Marriage

edit

Research demonstrates howchildhood abuseties into maladaptivecommunicationdynamics consisting of contempt-laden conflict andemotional withdrawal.These findings are important because maladaptive marital communication may be one mechanism by which traumatic childhood experiences translate into poor adult relationship quality. Forms of verbal aggression, such as contempt, belligerence, and defensiveness, are associated with destructive, hostile patterns of conflict resolution ([Gottman et al., 1998] and [Straus, 1979]). Couples who use such communication styles are more likely to have higher levels of marital distress (Roberts, 2000), lower levels of marital satisfaction (Holman and Jarvis, 2003), and lower levels of marital stability ([Gottman et al., 1998], [Holman and Jarvis, 2003] and [DeMaris, 2000]).[14]

Gottman (1999) identified several behaviors that are particularly indicative of distress in relationships. One series of behaviors, which he termed the "four horsemen", includes a cascading of responses such as expressing criticism, defensiveness, contempt,sarcasm,hostility,and withdrawal, the combination of which indicates a critical state of marriage dissolution.[15]

Carstensen, Gottman, and Levenson (1995) found that "Negative emotionalbehavior, such as expressed anger, sadness, contempt, and other negative emotions, appears to be the best discriminator between satisfied and dissatisfied marriages ". Carstensen, Gottman, and Levenson (1995) also discovered that" In terms of speaker behaviors, wives were coded as showing more total emotion, negative emotion, anger, joy, contempt, whining, and sadness. "This supports the stereotype that women express more emotion than men both in general and in relationships. It also supports the idea that men are less expressive than women and tend to be more defensive minded in conversations.[16]

Six short self-report measures were used to assess several component communication skills (Gottman 1999). Specifically, the questionnaires assessed Repair Attempts, Accepting Influence, Harsh Start-Up, Flooding, Gridlock, and the Four Horsemen. These six measures were chosen because they were of theoretical and clinical interest to the authors, incorporated both adaptive and maladaptive communication behaviors, and included those aspects of couple communication considered by many to be most toxic, including withdrawal and contempt (Gottman 1999; Gottman et al. 1998; Johnson 2003).[15]Finally, the Four Horsemen create a cascading sequence of responses in which one partner expresses criticism and the other partner responds with defensiveness, causing the first partner to react to the defensiveness with contempt, sarcasm, and/or hostility with their partner, eventually withdrawing from, or stonewalling, the conversation. This cascading negative sequence which occurs as a repetitive, interlocking pattern is believed to signify a critical end-stage process of relationship dissolution, representing a final common causal pathway to relationship dissolution (see Gottman 1994).[15]

In the bookBlink: The Power of Thinking Without Thinking,authorMalcolm Gladwelldiscusses John Gottman's theories of how to predict which couples will stay married. Gottman's theory states that there are four major emotional reactions that are destructive to a marriage: defensiveness, stonewalling, criticism, and contempt. Among these four, Gottman considers contempt the most destructive of them all.[17]For all other forms of aggression the Four Horsemen emerged as significant predictors of classification, which is expected given that this construct includes very negative, contemptuous behaviors. This is consistent with marital research, which contends that these communication behaviors are highly toxic, and erode relationship satisfaction (Cornelius et al. 2007; Gottman 1999).[15][17]

Power and distance

edit

People are likely to feel contempt towards a person perceived as low-status who is at a distant proximity, with other emotions resulting from different combinations of power and distance.[18]

Subject is Distant Subject is Close
Subject is perceived asPowerful Fear Envy
Subject is perceived asPowerless Contempt Compassion

See also

edit

References

edit
  1. ^TenHouten, W.D. (2007),A General Theory of Emotions and Social Life.Routledge.
  2. ^"Definition of CONTEMPT".merriam-webster.2024-01-20.Retrieved2024-01-26.
  3. ^Solomon R.C. (1993).The Passions: Emotions and the Meaning of Life.Hackett Publishing.
  4. ^"Contempt. (n.d.)".Online Etymology Dictionary.RetrievedApril 20,2008.
  5. ^ Ekman, P; Heider, K.G. (1988). "The Universality of Contempt Expression: A Replication".Motivation and Emotion.12(3): 303–308.doi:10.1007/bf00993116.S2CID143817186.
  6. ^ Google Books Ngram Viewer.
  7. ^Ekman, P. & Friesen, W. V (1969). The repertoire of nonverbal behavior: Categories, origins, usage, and encoding. Semiotica, 1, 49–98.
  8. ^Ekman; Heider (1988)."The Universality of a Contempt Expression: A Replication"(PDF).Motivation and Emotion.12(3): 303–308.doi:10.1007/bf00993116.S2CID143817186.
  9. ^abBell, M. (2005). "A Woman's Scorn: Toward a Feminist Defense of Contempt as a Moral Emotion".Hypatia.20(4): 80–93.doi:10.1111/j.1527-2001.2005.tb00537.x.S2CID143305885.
  10. ^Bell, M. (2013).Hard Feelings: The Moral Psychology of Contempt.Oxford University Press.ISBN0199794146
  11. ^Miller, C. H. (2035). How dare you! A measure of indignation. Manuscript in preparation, University of Oklahoma.
  12. ^abUnderwood, M. K.(2004). "III. Glares of Contempt, Eye Rolls of Disgust and Turning Away to Exclude: Non-Verbal Forms of Social Aggression among Girls".Feminism & Psychology.14(3): 371–375.doi:10.1177/0959353504044637.S2CID143410476.
  13. ^"City Council Allows Frowning",Daily Southtown,2003
  14. ^Krivickas, K. M.; Sanchez, L. A.; Kenney, C. T.; Wright, J. D. (2010). "Fiery wives and icy husbands: Pre-marital counseling and covenant marriage as buffers against effects of childhood abuse on gendered marital communication?".Social Science Research.39(5): 700.doi:10.1016/j.ssresearch.2010.05.003.
  15. ^abcdCornelius, T. L.; Shorey, R. C.; Beebe, S. M. (2010). "Self-Reported Communication Variables and Dating Violence: Using Gottman's Marital Communication Conceptualization".Journal of Family Violence.25(4): 439.doi:10.1007/s10896-010-9305-9.S2CID45452691.
  16. ^Carstensen, L. L.; Gottman, J. M.; Levenson, R. W. (1995). "Emotional behavior in long-term marriage".Psychology and Aging.10(1): 140–149.doi:10.1037/0882-7974.10.1.140.PMID7779311.
  17. ^abGladwell, Malcolm (2005).Blink.Back Bay Books imprint (Little, Brown and Company). pp. 32–33.ISBN978-0-316-01066-5.Archived fromthe originalon 2008-04-14.Retrieved2011-01-13.
  18. ^Beaumont, Leland R."Emotional Competency".emotionalcompetency.Retrieved7 June2017.
edit