Anearwormorbrainworm,[1]also described assticky musicorstuck song syndrome,[2]is acatchyor memorable piece of music or saying that continuously occupies a person's mind even after it is no longer being played or spoken about.[3][4]Involuntary Musical Imagery(INMI) is most common after earworms,[5][6][7][8][9]but INMI as a label is not solely restricted to earworms;musical hallucinationsalso fall into this category, although they are not the same thing.[6][10]Earworms are considered to be a common type ofinvoluntary cognition.[11]Some of the phrases often used to describe earworms include "musical imagery repetition" and "involuntary musical imagery".[1][12][13]
The wordearwormis acalquefrom the GermanOhrwurm.[14][15]The earliest known English usage is inDesmond Bagley's1978 novelFlyaway,where the author points out the German origin of his word.[16]
Researchers who have studied and written about the phenomenon includeTheodor Reik,[17]Sean Bennett,[18]Oliver Sacks,[1]Daniel Levitin,[19]James Kellaris,[20]Philip Beaman,[21]Vicky Williamson,[22]Diana Deutsch,[23]and, in a more theoretical perspective,Peter Szendy,[24]along with many more. The phenomenon is distinct frompalinacousis,a rare medical condition caused by damage to thetemporal lobeof the brain that results inauditory hallucinations.[25]
Incidence and causes
editResearcher Vicky Williamson atGoldsmiths, University of London,found in an uncontrolled study that earworms correlated with music exposure, but could also be triggered by experiences that trigger the memory of a song (involuntary memory) such as seeing a word that reminds one of the song, hearing a few notes from the song, or feeling an emotion one associates with the song. The list of songs collected in the study showed no particular pattern, other than popularity.[2]
According to research by James Kellaris, 98% of individuals experience earworms. Women and men experience the phenomenon equally often, but earworms tend to last longer for women and irritate them more.[26]Kellaris produced statistics suggesting that songs with lyrics may account for 73.7% of earworms, whereas instrumental music may cause only 7.7%.[27]
In 2010, published data in theBritish Journal of Psychologydirectly addressed the subject, and its results support earlier claims that earworms are usually 15 to 30 seconds in length and are more common in those with an interest in music.[21]Earworms can occur with either 'positive' or 'negative' music.[11]Positive music in this case is music that sounds happy and/or calm. Negative music is the opposite, where the music sounds angry or sad.
Earworms are not related only to music with lyrics; in a research experiment conducted by Ella Moeck and her colleagues in an attempt to find out if the positive/negative feeling of a piece of music affected earworms caused by that piece, they used only instrumental music.[11]Her experiment determined that all participants experienced a similar quantity of earworms, regardless of theemotional valence,although the quality of the earworm did vary. The earworms born from the negatively valenced music brought about more distress and occurred less frequently than those produced by positively valenced music.[11]
Antidotes
editScientists atWestern Washington Universityfound that engagingworking memoryin moderately difficult tasks such asanagrams,puzzles or reading was an effective way of stopping earworms and of reducing their recurrence.[28]Another publication points out that melodic music has a tendency to demonstrate repeating rhythm which may lead to endless repetition, unless a climax can be achieved to break the cycle.[29]
Research reported in 2015 by the School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences at theUniversity of Readingdemonstrated thatchewing gumcould help by similarly blocking the sub-vocal rehearsal component of auditory short-term or "working" memory associated with generating and manipulating auditory and musical images.[30]It has also been suggested to ask oneself why one is experiencing this particular song.[23]Another suggested remedy is to try to find a "cure song" to stop the repeating music.[31][32]
There are also so-called "cure songs" or "cure tunes" to get the earworm out of one's head. "God Save the King"is cited as a very popular and helpful choice of cure song.[33]"Happy Birthday"was also a popular choice in cure songs.[31]
Listening to the tune in a different/lower tempo or lower pitch, or a remixed version if it exists, can be an antidote. Listening to the tune from start to finish can also help. Since earworms are usually only a fragment of music, playing the tune all the way through can help break the loop.[34]
Notable cases
editJean Harris,who murderedHerman Tarnower,was obsessed with the song "Put the Blame on Mame"by Allan Roberts and Doris Fisher, which she first heard in the filmGilda(1946). She wouldrecall this regularlyfor over 33 years and could hold a conversation while playing it in her mind.[35]
In popular culture
editThis sectionneeds additional citations forverification.(May 2024) |
Mark Twain's 1876 story "A Literary Nightmare"(also known as" Punch, Brothers, Punch ") is about ajinglethat one can get rid of only by transferring it to another person.
In 1943Henry Kuttnerpublished the short story "Nothing but Gingerbread Left"about a song engineered to damage the Nazi war effort, culminating inAdolf Hitlerbeing unable to continue a speech.[36]
InAlfred Bester's 1953 novelThe Demolished Man,the protagonist uses a jingle specifically crafted to be a catchy, irritating nuisance as a tool to block mind readers from reading his mind.
InArthur C. Clarke's 1957 science fiction short story "The Ultimate Melody",a scientist, Gilbert Lister, develops the ultimate melody – one that so compels the brain that its listener becomes completely and forever enraptured by it. As the storyteller, Harry Purvis, explains, Lister theorized that a great melody" made its impression on the mind because it fitted in with the fundamental electrical rhythms going on in the brain. "Lister attempts to abstract from the hit tunes of the day to a melody that fits in so well with the electrical rhythms that it dominates them completely. He succeeds and is found in a catatonic state from which he never awakens.[37]
InFritz Leiber'sHugo Award-nominated short story "Rump-Titty-Titty-Tum-TAH-Tee" (1959), the title describes a rhythmic drumbeat so powerful that it rapidly spreads to all areas of human culture, until a counter-rhythm is developed that acts as an antidote.[38]
InJoe Simpson's 1988 bookTouching the Void,he talks about not being able to get the tune "Brown Girl in the Ring"byBoney Mout of his head. The book tells of his survival, against the odds, after a mountaineering accident in the remoteSiula Granderegion of South America. Alone, badly injured, and in a semi-delirious state, he is confused as to whether he is imagining the music or really hearing it.[39]
In theDexter's Laboratoryepisode titled "Head Band", a contagious group ofvirusesforce their host to sing what they are saying to the same "boy band" tune. The only way to be cured of the Boy Band Virus is for the viruses to break up and start their own solo careers.[40]
In theSpongeBob SquarePantsepisode titled “Earworm”, SpongeBob gets the “Musical Doodle” song stuck in his head, giving him an earworm, which ultimately turns out to be an actual worm, which is removed by his friends singing or playing other songs.
InThe Lego Movie 2: The Second Partthere is a scene in which most of the film's characters are subjected to "Catchy Song"and all except Lucy dance to it, while simultaneously the denizens of Harmony Town sing it to Emmet and Rex. Lucy/Wildstyle avoids being" brainwashed "by the song by breaking one of the speakers and using some of its pieces to build earmuffs for herself before escaping via air ducts, while Emmet and Rex escape in a similar fashion.
E. B. White's 1933satiricalshort story "The Supremacy of Uruguay" (reprinted inTimeless Stories for Today and Tomorrow) relates a fictional episode in the history of Uruguay where a powerful earworm is discovered in a popular American song. The Uruguayan military builds a squadron ofpilotless aircraftarmed withphonographsplaying a highly amplified recording of the earworm, and conquers the entire world by reducing the citizens of all nations to mindless insanity. "[T]he peoples were hopelessly mad, ravaged by an ineradicable noise... No one could hear anything except the noise in his own head."[41]
In 2014, musicianEmperor Xwrote a deliberately catchy song titled10,000-Year Earworm to Discourage Settlement Near Nuclear Waste Repositories (Don't Change Color, Kitty)in reference to the "ray cat"idea innuclear semiotics,attempting to embed a warning message in folklore that would still be remembered in 10,000 years' time.
Key characteristics
editAccording to research done in 2016 by Kelly Jakubowski and colleagues, published by theAmerican Psychological Association,there are certain characteristics that make songs more likely to become earworms. Earworm songs usually have a fast-paced tempo and an easy-to-remember melody. However, earworms also tend to have unusual intervals or repetitions that make them stand out from other songs. Earworms also tend to be played on the radio more than other songs and are usually featured at the top of the charts.[42]The chorus of a song is one of the most reported causes of earworms.[43]
The most frequently named earworms during this study were the following:
- "Bad Romance"byLady Gaga
- "Can't Get You Out of My Head"byKylie Minogue
- "Don't Stop Believin'"byJourney
- "Somebody That I Used to Know"byGotye
- "Moves like Jagger"byMaroon 5
- "California Gurls"byKaty Perry
- "Bohemian Rhapsody"byQueen
- "Alejandro"by Lady Gaga
- "Poker Face"by Lady Gaga
Susceptible traits
editKazumasa Negishi and Takahiro Sekiguchi did a study to see if there are specific traits that make a person more or less susceptible to earworms or involuntary musical imagery.[44]The participants in the study were assessed on obsessive-compulsive tendencies, theBig Five personality traits,and musical expertise. Negishi and Sekiguchi found that some of the obsessive-compulsive traits, such as intrusive thoughts, played a role in experiencing earworms while compulsive washing did not. In terms of the Big Five personality traits,neuroticismsignificantly predicted occurrences of earworms. Musical expertise created an effect of sophistication when it came to earworm occurrences.
Tools used in data gathering
editOne tool used to gather data on involuntary musical imagery (INMI)—and, more specifically, earworms—is called the Involuntary Musical Imagery Scale; it was created with the research compiled from George Floridou, Victoria Williamson, and Danial Müllensiefen. It uses four factors to measure different experiences surrounding earworms and INMI in general.[45]Those four factors include 'Negative Valence', 'Movement', 'Personal Reflections', and 'Help'.[45]Negative Valence is the category that measures the subjective response to the INMI experience.[45]Movement is a relatively new aspect to apply to INMI, it is essentially the INMI experience with accompanied embodied responses, which can include singing, humming, and dancing.[45]Personal Reflections is the occurrence of a personal quality, like unrelated thoughts, associated with the INMI; which are not directly related to the valence of the INMI itself.[45]Help is the category which determines the beneficial and constructive aspects to the INMI experiences, which could potentially reflect similarities in the characteristics of unfocused music listing and task-unrelated thought.[45]
See also
editReferences
edit- ^abcSacks, Oliver(2007).Musicophilia: Tales of Music and the Brain.First Vintage Books. pp. 41–48.ISBN978-1-4000-3353-9.
- ^abChatterjee, Rhitu (March 7, 2012)."Earworms: Why songs get stuck in our heads".BBC News.
- ^"Oxford Dictionaries:" earworm "".Oxford University Press. Archived fromthe originalon March 29, 2013.RetrievedJuly 4,2013.
- ^Halpern, Andrea R.; Bartlett, James C. (April 1, 2011)."The Persistence of Musical Memories: A Descriptive Study of Earworms".Music Perception.28(4): 425–432.doi:10.1525/mp.2011.28.4.425.ISSN0730-7829.
- ^Jakubowski, Kelly; Finkel, Sebastian; Stewart, Lauren; Müllensiefen, Daniel (2017)."Dissecting an earworm: Melodic features and song popularity predict involuntary musical imagery"(PDF).Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts.11(2). American Psychological Association (APA): 122–135.doi:10.1037/aca0000090.ISSN1931-390X.
- ^abWilliams, T. I. (2015)."The classification of involuntary musical imagery: The case for earworms"(PDF).Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, & Brain.15(1).American Psychological Association:5–13.doi:10.1037/pmu0000082.
- ^Williamson, Victoria J.; Jilka, Sagar R.; Fry, Joshua; Finkel, Sebastian; Müllensiefen, Daniel; Stewart, Lauren (September 27, 2011). "How do" earworms "start? Classifying the everyday circumstances of Involuntary Musical Imagery".Psychology of Music.40(3): 259–284.doi:10.1177/0305735611418553.S2CID145466099.
- ^Filippidi, I.; Timmers, R. (2017). "Relationships between everyday music listening habits and involuntary musical imagery: Does music listening condition musical imagery?".Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, & Brain.27(4).American Psychological Association:312–326.doi:10.1037/pmu0000194.S2CID149182669.
- ^Jakubowski, Kelly; Farrugia, Nicolas; Halpern, Andrea R.; Sankarpandi, Sathish K.; Stewart, Lauren (November 1, 2015)."The speed of our mental soundtracks: Tracking the tempo of involuntary musical imagery in everyday life".Memory & Cognition.43(8): 1229–1242.doi:10.3758/s13421-015-0531-5.ISSN1532-5946.PMC4624826.PMID26122757.
- ^Hemming, J.; Merrill, J. (2015). "On the distinction between involuntary musical imagery, musical hallucinosis, and musical hallucinations".Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, & Brain.25(4).American Psychological Association:435–442.doi:10.1037/pmu0000112.
- ^abcdMoeck, E. K.; Hyman, I. E; Takarangi, M. K. Y. (2018). "Understanding the overlap between positive and negative involuntary cognitions using instrumental earworms".Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, & Brain.28(3).American Psychological Association:164–177.doi:10.1037/pmu0000217.S2CID150180837.
- ^Liikkanen, L. A. (2012)."Inducing involuntary musical imagery: An experimental study"(PDF).Musicae Scientiae.16(2): 217–234.doi:10.1177/1029864912440770.S2CID146451325.
- ^Liikkanen, Lassi A. (2008)."Music in Everymind: Commonality of Involuntary Musical Imagery"(PDF).Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition (ICMPC 10).Sapporo, Japan: 408–412.ISBN978-4-9904208-0-2.Archived fromthe original(PDF)on February 3, 2014.
- ^"earworm"ArchivedOctober 15, 2014, at theWayback Machine,wordspy
- ^"Ohrwurm",dwds.de
- ^Kruszelnicki, Dr Karl (November 29, 2016)."The earworms you can't get out of your head".ABC Radio National.RetrievedMay 1,2022.
- ^Reik, Theodor (1953).The Haunting Melody: Psychoanalytic Experiences in Life and Music.New York: Grove Press.
- ^Bennett, Sean (August 30, 2002).Musical Imagery Repetition(Master). Cambridge University.
- ^Levitin, Daniel(2006).This Is Your Brain on Music: The Science of a Human Obsession.New York: Dutton, Penguin.ISBN0452288525.RetrievedAugust 7,2012.
- ^Kellaris, James J. (Winter 2001). "Identifying Properties of Tunes That Get 'Stuck in Your Head'".Proceedings of the Society for Consumer Psychology.Scottsdale, Arizona:American Psychological Society:66–67.
- ^abBeaman CP, Williams TI (November 2010)."Earworms (stuck song syndrome): towards a natural history of intrusive thoughts".British Journal of Psychology.101(Pt 4): 637–653.doi:10.1348/000712609X479636.PMID19948084.
- ^Chatterjee, Rhitu (March 6, 2012)."Earworms: Why songs get stuck in our heads".BBC News.RetrievedMarch 7,2012.
- ^abDeutsch, D.(2019). "Catchy Music and Earworms".Musical Illusions and Phantom Words: How Music and Speech Unlock Mysteries of the Brain.Oxford University Press. pp. 116–127.ISBN9780190206833.LCCN2018051786.
- ^Szendy, Peter (2012).Hits. Philosophy in the Jukebox.translated by William Bishop. Fordham University Press.
- ^Moore, David R.; Fuchs, Paul Paul Albert; Rees, Adrian; Palmer, Alan; Plack, Christopher J. (January 21, 2010).The Oxford Handbook of Auditory Science: The Auditory Brain.Oxford University Press. p. 535.ISBN9780199233281.RetrievedJuly 3,2013.
- ^Adams, Cecil(October 16, 2009),"Why do songs get stuck in your head?",The Straight Dope
- ^Hoffman, Carey (April 4, 2001)."Songs That Cause The Brain To 'Itch': UC Professor Investigating Why Certain Tunes Get Stuck In Our Heads".University of Cincinnati.RetrievedAugust 6,2012.
Of the 1,000 respondents, the kind of music respondents said they got stuck on most recently were songs with lyrics for 73.7 percent, jingles or ads for 18.6 percent and an instrumental tune for 7.7 percent.
- ^Gray, Richard (March 24, 2013)."Get that tune out of your head – scientists find how to get rid of earworms".The Daily Telegraph.Archived fromthe originalon March 24, 2013.RetrievedMarch 25,2013.
- ^Schwanauer, Stephan M.; Levitt, David A. (1993).Machine Models of Music.MIT Press. p.174.ISBN978-0-262-19319-1.
- ^"Listen up – new research shows chewing gum could remove that stuck record in your head",University of Reading, 22 April 2015
- ^ab"Science Identified 'Cure Songs' to Get Songs Unstuck From Your Brain, I Guess All Diseases Have Been Cured"by Dan Van Winkle,The Mary Sue,March 3, 2014
- ^Williamson VJ, Liikkanen LA, Jakubowski K, Stewart L (2014)."Sticky tunes: how do people react to involuntary musical imagery?".PLOS ONE.9(1): e86170.Bibcode:2014PLoSO...986170W.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0086170.PMC3908735.PMID24497938.
- ^"Music: How to get rid of an earworm".Durham University.RetrievedJuly 19,2020.,citing Williamson et al. 2014
- ^"How do you get rid of an earworm?".Australian Broadcasting Corporation.April 16, 2020.
- ^Díaz de Chumaceiro, Cora L. (October 16, 2004)."Jean Harris' Obsessive Film Song Recall".PsyArt.Archived fromthe originalon August 1, 2019.RetrievedDecember 11,2011.
- ^Kuttner, Henry."Nothing But Gingerbread Left".Archived fromthe originalon March 14, 2012.RetrievedOctober 10,2017.Full text of story
- ^Chorost, Michael,"The Ultimate Melody by Arthur C. Clarke",The Web site of aleph,archived fromthe originalon January 1, 2011,retrievedAugust 17,2010
- ^Pretor-Pinney, Gavin (2010),The Wavewatcher's Companion,Bloomsbury Publishing, p. 218,ISBN978-0-7475-8976-1
- ^Simpson, Joe (1988).Touching the Void.Harper & Row.ISBN9780060160272.
- ^"Dexter's Laboratory: Head Band / Stuffed Animal House / Used Ink".TV.Archived fromthe originalon February 26, 2021.RetrievedMay 29,2014.
- ^"The Supremacy of Uruguay".armandobronca. July 24, 2007. Archived fromthe originalon February 27, 2014.RetrievedJanuary 17,2014.
- ^"Psychologists Identify Key Characteristics of Earworms".American Psychological Association.November 3, 2016.RetrievedJuly 19,2020.
- ^Beaman, C. Philip; Williams, Tim I. (2010)."Earworms (stuck song syndrome): Towards a natural history of intrusive thoughts"(PDF).British Journal of Psychology.101(4): 637–653.doi:10.1348/000712609X479636.ISSN2044-8295.PMID19948084.
- ^Negishi, Kazumasa; Sekiguchi, Takahiro (June 4, 2020). Sudzina, Frantisek (ed.)."Individual traits that influence the frequency and emotional characteristics of involuntary musical imagery: An experience sampling study".PLOS ONE.15(6): e0234111.Bibcode:2020PLoSO..1534111N.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0234111.ISSN1932-6203.PMC7272041.PMID32497111.
- ^abcdefFloridou, G. A.; Williamson, V. J.; Stewart, L.; Müllensiefen, D. (2015)."The Involuntary Musical Imagery Scale (IMIS)"(PDF).Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, & Brain.25.American Psychological Association:28–36.doi:10.1037/pmu0000067.
Further reading
edit- Elizabeth Hellmuth Margulis (2013).On Repeat: How Music Plays the Mind.Oxford University Press.ISBN978-0199990825.
- Vadim Prokhorov (June 22, 2006)."Can't get it out of my head".The Guardian.
- Divya Singhal (December 8, 2011),Why this Kolaveri Di: Maddening Phenomenon of Earworm,SSRN1969781
External links
edit- The Involuntary Musical Imagery Scale (IMIS)at Goldsmith University of London
- Margulis, Elizabeth Hellmuth (January 16, 2014)."Why Songs Get Stuck in Your Head".The Atlantic.