Fearis an intensely unpleasant emotion in response toperceivingor recognizing a danger orthreat.Fear causes psychological changes that may producebehavioralreactions such as mounting an aggressive response or fleeing the threat. Fear in human beings may occur in response to a certainstimulusoccurring in the present, or inanticipationor expectation of a future threat perceived as ariskto oneself. The fear response arises from theperceptionofdangerleading to confrontation with or escape from/avoiding the threat (also known as thefight-or-flight response), which in extreme cases of fear (horror and terror) can be afreeze response.The fear response is also implicated in a number ofmental disorders,particularlyanxiety disorders.

The Man Made Mad with Fear,a painting byGustave Courbet

In humans and other animals, fear is modulated by the process ofcognitionand learning. Thus, fear is judged asrationaland appropriate, orirrationaland inappropriate (or unconscious). An irrational fear is called aphobia.

Fear is closely related to the emotionanxiety,which occurs as the result of often future threats that are perceived to be uncontrollable or unavoidable.[1]The fear response serves survival by engendering appropriate behavioral responses, so it has been preserved throughoutevolution.[2]Sociological and organizational research also suggests that individuals' fears are not solely dependent on their nature but are also shaped by their social relations and culture, which guide their understanding of when and how much fear to feel.[3][page needed]

Physiological signs

edit
A man showing signs of fear

Many physiological changes in the body are associated with fear, summarized as thefight-or-flight response.An innate response for coping with danger, it works by accelerating the breathing rate (hyperventilation), heart rate, vasoconstriction of the peripheral blood vessels leading to blood pooling, dilating the pupils, increasing muscle tension including the muscles attached to each hair follicle to contract and causing "goosebumps", or more clinically,piloerection(making a cold person warmer or a frightened animal look more impressive), sweating, increased blood glucose (hyperglycemia), increased serum calcium, increase in white blood cells called neutrophilic leukocytes, alertness leading to sleep disturbance and "butterflies in the stomach" (dyspepsia). This primitive mechanism may help an organism survive by either running away or fighting the danger.[4]With the series of physiological changes, the consciousness realizes an emotion of fear.

There are observable physical reactions in individuals who experience fear. An individual might experience a feeling of dizziness, lightheaded, like they are being choked, sweating, shortness of breath, vomiting or nausea, numbness or shaking and any other like symptoms. These bodily reactions informs the individual that they are afraid and should proceed to remove or get away from the stimulus that is causing that fear.[5]

Causes

edit

An influential categorization of stimuli causing fear was proposed by psychologistJeffrey Alan Gray;[6]namely,intensity,novelty,special evolutionary dangers, stimuli arising during social interaction, andconditionedstimuli.[7]Another categorization was proposed by Archer,[8]who, besides conditioned fear stimuli, categorized fear-evoking (as well asaggression-evoking) stimuli into three groups; namely,pain,novelty, andfrustration,although he also described "looming",which refers to an object rapidly moving towards the visual sensors of a subject, and can be categorized as" intensity ". Russell[9]described a more functional categorization of fear-evoking stimuli, in which for instance novelty is a variable affecting more than one category: 1) Predator stimuli (including movement, suddenness, proximity, but also learned and innate predator stimuli); 2) Physical environmental dangers (including intensity and heights); 3) Stimuli associated with increased risk of predation and other dangers (including novelty, openness, illumination, and being alone); 4) Stimuli stemming from conspecifics (including novelty, movement, and spacing behavior); 5) Species-predictable fear stimuli and experience (special evolutionary dangers); and 6) Fear stimuli that are not species predictable (conditioned fear stimuli).

Nature

edit
A prisoner atAbu Ghraibshows fear of a US army dog.

Although many fears are learned, the capacity to fear is part ofhuman nature.Many studies[10]have found that certain fears (e.g. animals, heights) are much more common than others (e.g. flowers, clouds). These fears are also easier to induce in the laboratory. This phenomenon is known aspreparedness.Because early humans that were quick to fear dangerous situations were more likely to survive and reproduce; preparedness is theorized to be a genetic effect that is the result ofnatural selection.[11]

From anevolutionary psychologyperspective, different fears may be differentadaptationsthat have been useful in our evolutionary past. They may have developed during different time periods. Some fears, such as fear of heights, may be common to allmammalsand developed during themesozoicperiod. Other fears, such as fear of snakes, may be common to allsimiansand developed during thecenozoictime period (the still-ongoing geological era encompassing the last 66 million of history). Still others, such as fear of mice and insects, may be unique to humans and developed during thepaleolithicandneolithictime periods (when mice and insects become important carriers of infectious diseases and harmful for crops and stored foods).[12]

Conditioning

edit

Nonhuman animals and humans innovate specific fears as a result of learning. This has been studied in psychology asfear conditioning,beginning with John B. Watson'sLittle Albert experimentin 1920, which was inspired after observing a child with an irrational fear of dogs. In this study, an 11-month-old boy was conditioned to fear a white rat in the laboratory. The fear became generalized to include other white, furry objects, such as a rabbit, dog, and even a Santa Claus mask with white cotton balls in the beard.

Fear can be learned by experiencing or watching a frighteningtraumaticaccident. For example, if a child falls into a well and struggles to get out, he or she may develop a fear of wells, heights (acrophobia), enclosed spaces (claustrophobia), or water (aquaphobia). There are studies looking at areas of the brain that are affected in relation to fear. When looking at these areas (such as theamygdala), it was proposed that a person learns to fear regardless of whether they themselves have experienced trauma, or if they have observed the fear in others. In a study completed by Andreas Olsson, Katherine I. Nearing and Elizabeth A. Phelps, the amygdala were affected both when subjects observed someone else being submitted to an aversive event, knowing that the same treatment awaited themselves, and when subjects were subsequently placed in a fear-provoking situation.[13]This suggests that fear can develop in both conditions, not just simply from personal history.

Fear is affected by cultural and historical context. For example, in the early 20th century, many Americans fearedpolio,a disease that can lead to paralysis.[14]There are consistent cross-cultural differences in how people respond to fear.[15]Display rulesaffect how likely people are to express the facial expression of fear and other emotions.

Fear ofvictimizationis a function of perceived risk and seriousness of potential harm.[16]

Common triggers

edit
"Fear", from Charles le Brun: "Caractères des passions",c. 1720

Phobias

edit

According to surveys, some of the most common fears are ofdemonsandghosts,the existence ofevilpowers,cockroaches,spiders,snakes,heights,water,enclosed spaces,tunnels,bridges,needles,social rejection,failure,examinations,andpublic speaking.[17][18][19]

Regionally some may more so fearterrorist attacks,death,war,criminalorgang violence,being alone,the future,nuclear war,[20]flying,clowns,intimacy,people,anddriving.[21]

Uncertainty

edit

Fear of the unknown orirrational fearis caused by negative thinking (worry) which arises fromanxietyaccompanied by a subjective sense of apprehension or dread.[22]Irrational fear shares a common neural pathway with other fears, a pathway that engages the nervous system to mobilize bodily resources in the face of danger or threat. Many people are scared of the "unknown". The irrational fear can branch out to many areas such as the hereafter, the next ten years or even tomorrow. Chronic irrational fear has deleterious effects since the elicitor stimulus is commonly absent or perceived from delusions. Such fear can createcomorbiditywith theanxiety disorderumbrella.[23]Being scared may cause people to experienceanticipatoryfear of what may lie ahead rather than planning and evaluating for the same. For example, "continuation of scholarly education" is perceived by many educators as a risk that may cause them fear and stress,[24]and they would rather teach things they've been taught than go and do research.[citation needed]

The ambiguity of situations that tend to be uncertain and unpredictable can cause anxiety in addition to other psychological and physical problems in some populations; especially those who engage it constantly, for example, in war-ridden places or in places of conflict, terrorism, abuse, etc. Poorparentingthat instills fear can also debilitate a child's psyche development or personality. For example, parents tell their children not to talk to strangers in order to protect them. In school, they would be motivated to not show fear in talking with strangers, but to be assertive and also aware of the risks and the environment in which it takes place. Ambiguous and mixed messages like this can affect their self-esteem and self-confidence. Researchers say talking to strangers is not something to be thwarted but allowed in a parent's presence if required.[25]Developing a sense ofequanimityto handle various situations is often advocated as an antidote to irrational fear and as an essential skill by a number of ancient philosophies.

Fear of the unknown (FOTU) "may be a, or possibly the, fundamental fear" from early times when there were many threats to life.[26]

Behavior

edit

Although fear behavior varies from species to species, it is often divided into two main categories; namely, avoidance/flight and immobility.[8]To these, different researchers have added different categories, such as threatdisplayand attack,[27]protective responses (includingstartleandloomingresponses),[28]defensive burying,[29]and social responses (including alarm vocalizations and submission).[27][30]Finally, immobility is often divided intofreezingandtonic immobility.[27][30]

The decision as to which particular fear behavior to perform is determined by the level of fear as well as the specific context, such as environmental characteristics (escape route present, distance to refuge), the presence of a discrete and localized threat, the distance between threat and subject, threat characteristics (speed, size, directness of approach), the characteristics of the subject under threat (size, physical condition, speed, degree ofcrypsis,protective morphological structures), social conditions (group size), and the amount of experience with the type of the threat.[7][8][30][31][32]

Mechanism

edit

Often laboratory studies with rats are conducted to examine the acquisition and extinction ofconditioned fearresponses.[33]In 2004, researchers conditioned rats (Rattus norvegicus) to fear a certain stimulus, through electric shock.[34]The researchers were able to then cause an extinction of this conditioned fear, to a point that no medications or drugs were able to further aid in the extinction process. The rats showed signs of avoidance learning, not fear, but simply avoiding the area that brought pain to the test rats. The avoidance learning of rats is seen as aconditioned response,and therefore the behavior can be unconditioned, as supported by the earlier research.

Species-specific defense reactions (SSDRs) oravoidance learningin nature is the specific tendency to avoid certain threats or stimuli, it is how animals survive in the wild. Humans and animals both share these species-specific defense reactions, such as the flight-or-fight, which also include pseudo-aggression, fake or intimidating aggression and freeze response to threats, which is controlled by thesympathetic nervous system.These SSDRs are learned very quickly through social interactions between others of the same species, other species, and interaction with the environment.[35]These acquired sets of reactions or responses are not easily forgotten. The animal that survives is the animal that already knows what to fear and how to avoid this threat. An example in humans is the reaction to the sight of a snake, many jump backwards before cognitively realizing what they are jumping away from, and in some cases, it is a stick rather than a snake.

As with many functions of the brain, there are various regions of the brain involved in deciphering fear in humans and other nonhuman species.[36]Theamygdalacommunicates both directions between theprefrontal cortex,hypothalamus,thesensory cortex,thehippocampus,thalamus,septum,and thebrainstem.The amygdala plays an important role in SSDR, such as the ventral amygdalofugal, which is essential forassociative learning,and SSDRs are learned through interaction with the environment and others of the same species. An emotional response is created only after the signals have been relayed between the different regions of the brain, and activating the sympathetic nervous systems; which controls theflight, fight, freeze, fright, and faint response.[37][38]Often a damaged amygdala can cause impairment in the recognition of fear (like the human case ofpatient S.M.).[39]This impairment can cause different species to lack the sensation of fear, and often can become overly confident, confronting larger peers, or walking up to predatory creatures.

Robert C. Bolles(1970), a researcher at University of Washington, wanted to understand species-specific defense reactions and avoidance learning among animals, but found that the theories of avoidance learning and the tools that were used to measure this tendency were out of touch with the natural world.[40]He theorized the species-specific defense reaction (SSDR).[41]There are three forms of SSDRs: flight, fight (pseudo-aggression), or freeze. Even domesticated animals have SSDRs, and in those moments it is seen that animals revert to atavistic standards and become "wild" again. Dr. Bolles states that responses are often dependent on the reinforcement of a safety signal, and not the aversive conditioned stimuli. This safety signal can be a source of feedback or even stimulus change. Intrinsic feedback or information coming from within, muscle twitches, increased heart rate, are seen to be more important in SSDRs than extrinsic feedback, stimuli that comes from the external environment. Dr. Bolles found that most creatures have some intrinsic set of fears, to help assure survival of the species. Rats will run away from anyshockingevent, and pigeons will flap their wings harder when threatened. The wing flapping in pigeons and the scattered running of rats are considered species-specific defense reactions or behaviors. Bolles believed that SSDRs are conditioned throughPavlovianconditioning, and not operant conditioning; SSDRs arise from the association between the environmental stimuli and adverse events.[42]Michael S. Fanselowconducted an experiment, to test some specific defense reactions, he observed that rats in two different shock situations responded differently, based on instinct or defensive topography, rather than contextual information.[43]

Species-specific defense responses are created out of fear, and are essential for survival.[44]Rats that lack the genestathminshow no avoidance learning, or a lack of fear, and will often walk directly up to cats and be eaten.[45]Animals use these SSDRs to continue living, to help increase their chance offitness,by surviving long enough to procreate. Humans and animals alike have created fear to know what should be avoided, and this fear can be learned throughassociationwith others in the community, or learned through personal experience with a creature, species, or situations that should be avoided. SSDRs are an evolutionary adaptation that has been seen in many species throughout the world including rats,chimpanzees,prairie dogs,and evenhumans,an adaptation created to help individual creatures survive in a hostile world.

Fear learning changes across the lifetime due to natural developmental changes in the brain.[46][47]This includes changes in theprefrontal cortexand theamygdala.[48]

The visual exploration of an emotional face does not follow a fixed pattern but modulated by the emotional content of the face. Scheller et al.[49]found that participants paid more attention to the eyes when recognising fearful or neutral faces, while the mouth was fixated on when happy faces are presented, irrespective of task demands and spatial locations of face stimuli. These findings were replicated when fearful eyes are presented[50]and when canonical face configurations are distorted for fearful, neutral and happy expressions.[51]

Neurocircuitry in mammals

edit
  • The thalamus collects sensory data from the senses
  • Sensory cortex receives data from the thalamus and interprets it
  • Sensory cortex organizes information for dissemination to the hypothalamus (fight or flight), amygdalae (fear), hippocampus (memory)

The brain structures that are the center of most neurobiological events associated with fear are the twoamygdalae,located behind the pituitary gland. Each amygdala is part of a circuitry of fear learning.[2]They are essential for proper adaptation to stress and specific modulation of emotional learning memory. In the presence of a threatening stimulus, the amygdalae generate the secretion of hormones that influence fear and aggression.[52]Once a response to the stimulus in the form of fear or aggression commences, the amygdalae may elicit the release of hormones into the body to put the person into a state of alertness, in which they are ready to move, run, fight, etc. This defensive response is generally referred to in physiology as thefight-or-flight responseregulated by the hypothalamus, part of thelimbic system.[53]Once the person is in safe mode, meaning that there are no longer any potential threats surrounding them, the amygdalae will send this information to the medialprefrontal cortex(mPFC) where it is stored for similar future situations, which is known asmemory consolidation.[54]

Some of the hormones involved during the state of fight-or-flight includeepinephrine,which regulates heart rate and metabolism as well as dilating blood vessels and air passages,norepinephrineincreasing heart rate, blood flow to skeletal muscles and the release of glucose from energy stores,[55]andcortisolwhich increases blood sugar, increases circulating neutrophilic leukocytes, calcium amongst other things.[56]

After a situation which incites fear occurs, the amygdalae andhippocampusrecord the event through synapticplasticity.[57]The stimulation to the hippocampus will cause the individual to remember many details surrounding the situation.[58]Plasticity and memory formation in the amygdala are generated by activation of the neurons in the region. Experimental data supports the notion that synaptic plasticity of the neurons leading to the lateral amygdalae occurs with fear conditioning.[59]In some cases, this forms permanent fear responses such aspost-traumatic stress disorder(PTSD) or aphobia.[60]MRI and fMRI scans have shown that the amygdalae in individuals diagnosed with such disorders includingbipolarorpanic disorderare larger and wired for a higher level of fear.[61]

Pathogens can suppress amygdala activity. Rats infected with thetoxoplasmosisparasite become less fearful of cats, sometimes even seeking out their urine-marked areas. This behavior often leads to them being eaten by cats. The parasite then reproduces within the body of the cat. There is evidence that the parasite concentrates itself in the amygdala of infected rats.[62]In a separate experiment, rats with lesions in the amygdala did not express fear or anxiety towards unwanted stimuli. These rats pulled on levers supplying food that sometimes sent out electrical shocks. While they learned to avoid pressing on them, they did not distance themselves from these shock-inducing levers.[63]

Several brain structures other than the amygdalae have also been observed to be activated when individuals are presented with fearful vs. neutral faces, namely the occipitocerebellarregions including thefusiform gyrusand theinferior parietal/superior temporalgyri.[64]Fearful eyes, brows and mouth seem to separately reproduce these brain responses.[64]Scientists from Zurich studies show that the hormone oxytocin related to stress and sex reduces activity in your brain fear center.[65]

Pheromones and contagion

edit

In threatening situations, insects, aquatic organisms, birds, reptiles, and mammals emit odorant substances, initially called alarm substances, which are chemical signals now called alarmpheromones.This is to defend themselves and at the same time to inform members of the same species of danger and leads to observable behavior change like freezing, defensive behavior, or dispersion depending on circumstances and species. For example, stressed rats release odorant cues that cause other rats to move away from the source of the signal.

After the discovery of pheromones in 1959, alarm pheromones were first described in 1968 in ants[66]and earthworms,[67]and four years later also found in mammals, both mice and rats.[68]Over the next two decades, identification and characterization of these pheromones proceeded in all manner of insects and sea animals, including fish, but it was not until 1990 that more insight into mammalian alarm pheromones was gleaned.

In 1985, a link between odors released by stressed rats andpain perceptionwas discovered: unstressed rats exposed to these odors developed opioid-mediated analgesia.[69]In 1997, researchers found that bees became less responsive to pain after they had been stimulated withisoamyl acetate,a chemical smelling of banana, and a component of bee alarm pheromone.[70]The experiment also showed that the bees' fear-inducedpain tolerancewas mediated by anendorphin.

By using theforced swimming testin rats as a model of fear-induction, the first mammalian "alarm substance" was found.[71]In 1991, this "alarm substance" was shown to fulfill criteria for pheromones: well-defined behavioral effect, species specificity, minimal influence of experience and control for nonspecific arousal. Rat activity testing with the alarm pheromone, and their preference/avoidance for odors from cylinders containing the pheromone, showed that the pheromone had very lowvolatility.[72]

In 1993 a connection between alarm chemosignals in mice and theirimmune responsewas found.[73]Pheromone production in mice was found to be associated with or mediated by thepituitary glandin 1994.[74]

In 2004, it was demonstrated that rats' alarm pheromones had different effects on the "recipient" rat (the rat perceiving the pheromone) depending which body region they were released from: Pheromone production from the face modified behavior in the recipient rat, e.g. caused sniffing or movement, whereas pheromone secreted from the rat's anal area inducedautonomic nervous systemstress responses, like an increase in core body temperature.[75]Further experiments showed that when a rat perceived alarm pheromones, it increased its defensive and risk assessment behavior,[76]and its acousticstartle reflexwas enhanced.

It was not until 2011 that a link between severe pain, neuroinflammation and alarm pheromones release in rats was found: real timeRT-PCRanalysis of rat brain tissues indicated that shocking the footpad of a rat increased its production ofproinflammatory cytokinesin deep brain structures, namely ofIL-1β,heteronuclearCorticotropin-releasing hormoneandc-fosmRNA expressions in both theparaventricular nucleusand the bed nucleus of thestria terminalis,and it increased stress hormone levels in plasma (corticosterone).[77]

Theneurocircuitfor how rats perceive alarm pheromones was shown to be related to thehypothalamus,brainstem,andamygdalae,all of which are evolutionary ancient structures deep inside or in the case of the brainstem underneath the brain away from the cortex, and involved in thefight-or-flight response,as is the case in humans.[78]

Alarm pheromone-induced anxiety in rats has been used to evaluate the degree to whichanxiolyticscan alleviate anxiety in humans. For this, the change in theacoustic startle reflexof rats with alarm pheromone-induced anxiety (i.e. reduction of defensiveness) has been measured. Pretreatment of rats with one of five anxiolytics used in clinical medicine was able to reduce their anxiety: namelymidazolam,phenelzine(a nonselective monoamine oxidase (MAO) inhibitor),propranolol,a nonselectivebeta blocker,clonidine,anAlpha 2 adrenergic agonistorCP-154,526,acorticotropin-releasing hormone antagonist.[79]

Faulty development of odor discrimination impairs theperceptionof pheromones and pheromone-related behavior, likeaggressive behaviorand mating in male rats: The enzymeMitogen-activated protein kinase 7(MAPK7) has been implicated in regulating the development of the olfactory bulb and odor discrimination and it is highly expressed in developing rat brains, but absent in most regions of adult rat brains.Conditional deletionof the MAPK7gene in mouse neural stem cells impairs several pheromone-mediated behaviors, including aggression and mating in male mice. These behavior impairments were not caused by a reduction in the level of testosterone, by physical immobility, by heightened fear or anxiety or by depression. Using mouse urine as a natural pheromone-containing solution, it has been shown that the impairment was associated with defective detection of related pheromones, and with changes in their inborn preference for pheromones related to sexual and reproductive activities.[80]

Lastly, alleviation of an acute fear response because a friendly peer (or in biological language: an affiliativeconspecific)tends and befriendsis called "social buffering".The term is in analogy to the 1985" buffering "hypothesis in psychology, wheresocial supporthas been proven to mitigate the negative health effects of alarm pheromone mediated distress.[81]The role of a "social pheromone" is suggested by the recent discovery that olfactory signals are responsible in mediating the "social buffering" in male rats.[82]"Social buffering" was also observed to mitigate the conditioned fear responses of honeybees. A bee colony exposed to an environment of high threat of predation did not show increased aggression and aggressive-like gene expression patterns in individual bees, but decreased aggression. That the bees did not simplyhabituateto threats is suggested by the fact that the disturbed colonies also decreased their foraging.[83]

Biologists have proposed in 2012 that fear pheromones evolved as molecules of "keystone significance", a term coined in analogy tokeystone species.Pheromones may determinespecies compositionsand affect rates of energy and material exchange in anecological community.Thus pheromones generate structure in afood weband play critical roles in maintainingnatural systems.[84]

Humans

edit

Evidence of chemosensory alarm signals in humans has emerged slowly: Although alarm pheromones have not been physically isolated and their chemical structures have not been identified in humans so far, there is evidence for their presence.Androstadienone,for example, a steroidal, endogenous odorant, is a pheromone candidate found in human sweat, axillary hair and plasma. The closely related compoundandrostenoneis involved in communicating dominance, aggression or competition; sex hormone influences on androstenone perception in humans showed a high testosterone level related to heightened androstenone sensitivity in men, a high testosterone level related tounhappinessin response to androstenone in men, and a high estradiol level related to disliking of androstenone in women.[85]

A German study from 2006 showed when anxiety-induced versus exercise-induced human sweat from a dozen people was pooled and offered to seven study participants, of five able to olfactorily distinguish exercise-induced sweat from room air, three could also distinguish exercise-induced sweat from anxiety induced sweat. Theacoustic startle reflexresponse to a sound when sensing anxiety sweat was larger than when sensing exercise-induced sweat, as measured byelectromyographyanalysis of the orbital muscle, which is responsible for the eyeblink component. This showed for the first time that fear chemosignals can modulate the startle reflex in humans without emotional mediation; fear chemosignals primed the recipient's "defensive behavior" prior to the subjects' conscious attention on the acoustic startle reflex level.[86]

In analogy to the social buffering of rats and honeybees in response to chemosignals, induction ofempathyby "smelling anxiety" of another person has been found in humans.[87]

A study from 2013 provided brain imaging evidence that human responses to fear chemosignals may begender-specific.Researchers collected alarm-induced sweat and exercise-induced sweat from donors extracted it, pooled it and presented it to 16 unrelated people undergoing functional brainMRI.While stress-induced sweat from males produced a comparably strong emotional response in both females and males, stress-induced sweat from females produced markedly stronger arousal in women than in men. Statistical tests pinpointed this gender-specificity to the right amygdala and strongest in the superficial nuclei. Since no significant differences were found in theolfactory bulb,the response to female fear-induced signals is likely based on processing the meaning, i.e. on the emotional level, rather than the strength of chemosensory cues from each gender, i.e. the perceptual level.[88]

Anapproach-avoidance taskwas set up where volunteers seeing either an angry or a happy cartoon face on a computer screen pushed away or pulled toward them a joystick as fast as possible. Volunteers smelling androstadienone, masked with clove oil scent responded faster, especially to angry faces than those smelling clove oil only, which was interpreted as androstadienone-related activation of the fear system.[89]A potential mechanism of action is, thatandrostadienonealters the "emotional face processing". Androstadienone is known to influence the activity of thefusiform gyruswhich is relevant forface recognition.

Cognitive-consistency theory

edit

Cognitive-consistencytheories assume that "when two or more simultaneously active cognitive structures are logically inconsistent, arousal is increased, which activates processes with the expected consequence of increasing consistency and decreasing arousal."[90]In this context, it has been proposed that fear behavior is caused by an inconsistency between a preferred, or expected, situation and the actually perceived situation, and functions to remove the inconsistent stimulus from the perceptual field, for instance by fleeing or hiding, thereby resolving the inconsistency.[90][91][8]This approach puts fear in a broader perspective, also involvingaggressionandcuriosity.When the inconsistency between perception and expectancy is small, learning as a result of curiosity reduces inconsistency by updating expectancy to match perception. If the inconsistency is larger, fear or aggressive behavior may be employed to alter the perception in order to make it match expectancy, depending on the size of the inconsistency as well as the specific context. Aggressive behavior is assumed to alter perception by forcefully manipulating it into matching the expected situation, while in some cases thwarted escape may also trigger aggressive behavior in an attempt to remove the thwarting stimulus.[90]

Research

edit

In order to improve our understanding of the neural and behavioral mechanisms of adaptive and maladaptive fear, investigators use a variety of translational animal models.[92]These models are particularly important for research that would be too invasive for human studies. Rodents such as mice and rats are common animal models, but other species are used. Certain aspects of fear research still requires more research such as sex, gender, and age differences.

Models

edit

These animal models include, but are not limited to, fear conditioning, predator-based psychosocial stress,single prolonged stress, chronic stress models, inescapable foot/tail shocks, immobilization or restraint, and stress enhanced fear learning. While the stress and fear paradigms differ between the models, they tend to involve aspects such as acquisition, generalization, extinction, cognitive regulation, and reconsolidation.[93][94]

Pavlovian

edit

Fear conditioning, also known as Pavlovian or classical conditioning, is a process of learning that involves pairing a neutral stimulus with an unconditional stimulus (US).[95]A neutral stimulus is something like a bell, tone, or room that does not elicit a response normally where a US is a stimulus that results in a natural or unconditioned response (UR) – in Pavlov's famous experiment the neutral stimulus is a bell and the US would be food with the dog's salvation being the UR. Pairing the neutral stimulus and the US results in the UR occurring not only with the US but also the neutral stimulus. When this occurs the neutral stimulus is referred to as the conditional stimulus (CS) and the response the conditional response (CR). In the fear conditioning model of Pavlovian conditioning the US is an aversive stimulus such as a shock, tone, or unpleasant odor.

Predator-based psychosocial stress

edit

Predator-based psychosocial stress (PPS) involves a more naturalistic approach to fear learning.[96]Predators such as a cat, a snake, or urine from a fox or cat are used along with other stressors such as immobilization or restraint in order to generate instinctual fear responses.[97]

Chronic stress models

edit

Chronic stress models include chronic variable stress, chronic social defeat, and chronic mild stress.[96][98]These models are often used to study how long-term or prolonged stress/pain can alter fear learning and disorders.[96][99]

Single prolonged stress

edit

Single prolonged stress (SPS) is a fear model that is often used to study PTSD.[100][101]Its paradigm involves multiple stressors such as immobilization, a force swim, and exposure to ether delivered concurrently to the subject.[101]This is used to study non-naturalistic, uncontrollable situations that can cause a maladaptive fear responses that is seen in a lot of anxiety and traumatic based disorders.

Stress enhanced fear learning

edit

Stress enhanced fear learning (SEFL) like SPS is often used to study the maladaptive fear learning involved in PTSD and other traumatic based disorders.[96][102]SEFL involves a single extreme stressor such as a large number of footshocks simulating a single traumatic stressor that somehow enhances and alters future fear learning.[96][103][104]

Management

edit

Pharmaceutical

edit

A drug treatment for fear conditioning and phobias via the amygdalae is the use ofglucocorticoids.[105]In one study, glucocorticoid receptors in the central nuclei of the amygdalae were disrupted in order to better understand the mechanisms of fear and fear conditioning. The glucocorticoid receptors were inhibited using lentiviral vectors containing Cre-recombinase injected into mice. Results showed that disruption of the glucocorticoid receptors prevented conditioned fear behavior. The mice were subjected to auditory cues which caused them to freeze normally. A reduction of freezing was observed in the mice that had inhibited glucocorticoid receptors.[106]

Psychological

edit

Cognitive behavioral therapyhas been successful in helping people overcome their fear. Because fear is more complex than just forgetting or deletingmemories,an active and successful approach involves people repeatedly confronting their fears. By confronting their fears in a safe manner a person can suppress the "fear-triggering memories" or stimuli.[107]

Exposure therapyhas known to have helped up to 90% of people with specificphobiasto significantly decrease their fear over time.[54][107]

Another psychological treatment issystematic desensitization,which is a type of behavior therapy used to completely remove the fear or produce a disgusted response to this fear and replace it. The replacement that occurs will be relaxation and will occur through conditioning. Through conditioning treatments, muscle tensioning will lessen and deep breathing techniques will aid in de-tensioning.

Literary and religious

edit

There are other methods for treating or coping with one's fear, such as writing down rational thoughts regarding fears. Journal entries are a healthy method of expressing one's fears without compromising safety or causing uncertainty. Another suggestion is a fear ladder. To create a fear ladder, one must write down all of their fears and score them on a scale of one to ten. Next, the person addresses their phobia, starting with the lowest number.

Religion can help some individuals cope with fear.[108]

Incapability

edit

People who have damage to theiramygdalae,which can be caused by a rare genetic disease known asUrbach–Wiethe disease,are unable to experience fear. The disease destroys both amygdalae in late childhood. Since the discovery of the disease, there have only been 400 recorded cases. A lack of fear can allow someone to get into a dangerous situation they otherwise would have avoided.[109]

Society and culture

edit
Painting byGuido Renic. 1611

Death

edit

The fear of the end of life and its existence is, in other words, the fear of death. Historically, attempts were made to reduce this fear by performing rituals which have helped collect the cultural ideas that we now have in the present.[citation needed]These rituals also helped preserve the cultural ideas. The results and methods of human existence had been changing at the same time that social formation was changing.

When people are faced with their own thoughts of death, they either accept that they are dying or will die because they have lived a full life or they will experience fear. A theory was developed in response to this, which is called theterror management theory.The theory states that a person's cultural worldviews (religion, values, etc.) will mitigate the terror associated with the fear of death through avoidance. To help manage their terror, they find solace in their death-denying beliefs, such as their religion. Another way people cope with their death related fears is pushing any thoughts of death into the future or by avoiding these thoughts all together through distractions.[110]Although there are methods for one coping with the terror associated with their fear of death, not everyone suffers from these same uncertainties. People who believe they have lived life to the "fullest" typically do not fear death.

Death anxiety is multidimensional; it covers "fears related to one's own death, the death of others, fear of the unknown after death, fear of obliteration, and fear of the dying process, which includes fear of a slow death and a painful death".[111]

TheYalephilosopherShelly Kaganexamined fear of death in a 2007 Yale open course[112]by examining the following questions: Is fear of death a reasonable appropriate response? What conditions are required and what are appropriate conditions for feeling fear of death? What is meant by fear, and how much fear is appropriate? According to Kagan for fear in general to make sense, three conditions should be met:

  1. the object of fear needs to be "something bad"
  2. there needs to be a non-negligible chance that the bad state of affairs will happen
  3. there needs to be some uncertainty about the bad state of affairs

The amount of fear should be appropriate to the size of "the bad". If the three conditions are not met, fear is an inappropriate emotion. He argues, that death does not meet the first two criteria, even if death is a "deprivation of good things" and even if one believes in a painful afterlife. Because death is certain, it also does not meet the third criterion, but he grants that the unpredictability of when one diesmaybe cause to a sense of fear.[112]

In a 2003 study of 167 women and 121 men, aged 65–87, lowself-efficacypredicted fear of the unknown after death and fear of dying for women and men better than demographics, social support, and physical health. Fear of death was measured by a "Multidimensional Fear of Death Scale" which included the 8 subscales Fear of Dying, Fear of the Dead, Fear of Being Destroyed, Fear for Significant Others, Fear of the Unknown, Fear of Conscious Death, Fear for the Body After Death, and Fear of Premature Death. Inhierarchical multiple regressionanalysis, the most potent predictors of death fears were low "spiritual health efficacy", defined as beliefs relating to one's perceived ability to generate spiritually based faith and inner strength, and low "instrumental efficacy", defined as beliefs relating to one's perceived ability to manage activities of daily living.[111]

Psychologists have tested the hypotheses that fear of death motivates religious commitment, and that assurances about an afterlife alleviate the fear, with equivocal results.[citation needed]Religiosity can be related to fear of death when the afterlife is portrayed as time of punishment. "Intrinsic religiosity", as opposed to mere "formal religious involvement", has been found to be negatively correlated with death anxiety.[111]In a 1976 study of people of various Christian denominations, those who were most firm in their faith, who attended religious services weekly, were the least afraid of dying. The survey found a negative correlation between fear of death and "religious concern".[113][better source needed]

In a 2006 study of white, Christian men and women the hypothesis was tested that traditional, church-centered religiousness and de-institutionalized spiritual seeking are ways of approaching fear of death in old age. Both religiousness and spirituality were related to positive psychosocial functioning, but only church-centered religiousness protected subjects against the fear of death.[114][115][better source needed]

Religion

edit

Statiusin theThebaid(Book 3, line 661) aired the irreverent suggestion that "fear first made gods in the world".[116]

From a Christian theological perspective, the wordfearcan encompass more than simple dread. Robert B. Strimple says that fear includes the "... convergence of awe, reverence, adoration, humility..".[117]Sometranslations of the Bible,such as theNew International Version,sometimes express the concept offearwith the wordreverence.

A similar phrase, "God-fearing", is sometimes used as a rough synonym for "pious". It is a standard translation for theArabicwordtaqwa(Arabic:تقوى;"forbearance, restraint"[118]) inMuslimcontexts.[119]InJudaism,"fear of God" describes obedience toJewish laweven when invisible to others.[120]

Manipulation

edit

Fear may be politically and culturally manipulated to persuade citizenry of ideas which would otherwise be widely rejected or dissuade citizenry from ideas which would otherwise be widely supported. In contexts of disasters, nation-states manage the fear not only to provide their citizens with an explanation about the event or blaming some minorities, but also to adjust their previous beliefs.

Fear can alter how a person thinks or reacts to situations because fear has the power to inhibit one's rational way of thinking. As a result, people who do not experience fear, are able to use fear as a tool to manipulate others. People who are experiencing fear, seek preservation through safety and can be manipulated by a person who is there to provide that safety that is being sought after. "When we're afraid, a manipulator can talk us out of the truth we see right in front of us. Words become more real than reality"[121]By this, a manipulator can use our fear to manipulate us out the truth and instead make us believe and trust in their truth. Politicians are notorious for using fear to manipulate the people into supporting their policies.This strategy taps into primal human emotions, leveraging fear of the unknown, external threats, or perceived dangers to influence decision-making.[122]

Fiction and mythology

edit
A still from the filmCarnival of Souls

Fear is found and reflected inmythologyand folklore as well as in works offictionsuch as novels and films.

Works of dystopian and (post)apocalyptic fiction convey the fears and anxieties of societies.[123][124]

The fear ofthe world's endis about as old as civilization itself.[125]In a 1967 study,Frank Kermodesuggests that the failure of religious prophecies led to a shift in how society apprehends this ancient mode.[126]Scientific and critical thought supplanting religious andmythical thoughtas well as a public emancipation may be the cause of eschatology becoming replaced by more realistic scenarios. Such might constructively provoke discussion and steps to be taken to prevent depictedcatastrophes.

The Story of the Youth Who Went Forth to Learn What Fear Wasis a German fairy tale dealing with the topic of not knowing fear. Many stories also include characters who fear the antagonist of the plot. One important characteristic of historical and mythicalheroesacross cultures is to be fearless in the face of big and often lethal enemies.[citation needed]

The Magnus Archivesis a fiction horror podcast written by Jonathan Sims and directed by Alexander J. Newall that, among other things, formulates anarchetypalontologyof fear through the dissemination of case files at aparanormalresearch institute set in a world where themetaphysicalbasis of paranormal activity and unexplainable horrors is fear incarnate.[127]The diegesis states that true categorization of fear is impossible, that fear is all one unknowable thing;[128]however, there exists an ontological structure of fear archetypes in this universe proposed by a fictional version of the architectRobert Smirke.It is a unique construction of fear in that it is not based on the science or neurology of fear, but on thematic and experiential connections between different phobias. For example, the fear of disease and vermin comes from the same place as the fear of abusive relationships, as both lie in fearing corruptions to the self.[129][130]The final season of the podcast consists almost entirely ofpoeticmeditations on the nature of fear.

Athletics

edit

In the world of athletics, fear is often used as a means of motivation to not fail.[131]This situation involves using fear in a way that increases the chances of a positive outcome. In this case, the fear that is being created is initially a cognitive state to the receiver.[132]This initial state is what generates the first response of the athlete, this response generates a possibility of fight or flight reaction by the athlete (receiver), which in turn will increase or decrease the possibility of success or failure in the certain situation for the athlete.[133]The amount of time that the athlete has to determine this decision is small but it is still enough time for the receiver to make a determination through cognition.[132]Even though the decision is made quickly, the decision is determined through past events that have been experienced by the athlete.[134]The results of these past events will determine how the athlete will make his cognitive decision in the split second that he or she has.[131]

Fear of failure as described above has been studied frequently in the field of sport psychology. Many scholars have tried to determine how often fear of failure is triggered within athletes, as well as what personalities of athletes most often choose to use this type of motivation. Studies have also been conducted to determine the success rate of this method of motivation.

Murray's Exploration in Personal (1938) was one of the first studies that actually identified fear of failure as an actual motive to avoid failure or to achieve success. His studies suggested that inavoidance, the need to avoid failure, was found in many college-aged men during the time of his research in 1938.[135]This was a monumental finding in the field of psychology because it allowed other researchers to better clarify how fear of failure can actually be a determinant of creating achievement goals as well as how it could be used in the actual act of achievement.[136]

In the context of sport, a model was created by R.S. Lazarus in 1991 that uses the cognitive-motivational-relational theory of emotion.[132]

It holds that Fear of Failure results when beliefs or cognitive schemas about aversive consequences of failing are activated by situations in which failure is possible. These belief systems predispose the individual to make appraisals of threat and experience the state anxiety that is associated with Fear of Failure in evaluative situations.[136][132]

Another study was done in 2001 by Conroy, Poczwardowski, and Henschen that created five aversive consequences of failing that have been repeated over time. The five categories include (a) experiencing shame and embarrassment, (b) devaluing one's self-estimate, (c) having an uncertain future, (d) important others losing interest, (e) upsetting important others.[131]These five categories can help one infer the possibility of an individual to associate failure with one of these threat categories, which will lead them to experiencing fear of failure.

In summary, the two studies that were done above created a more precise definition of fear of failure, which is "a dispositional tendency to experience apprehension and anxiety in evaluative situations because individuals have learned that failure is associated with aversive consequences".[136]

The author andinternet content creatorJohn Greenwrote about “the yips”—a common colloquialism for a debilitating, often chronic manifestation of athletic anxiety experienced by some professional athletes—in an essay for hispodcastand novelThe Anthropocene Reviewed.[137]Green discusses famous examples of when athletic anxiety has ruined careers and juxtaposes it with the nature ofgeneral anxietyas a whole. Green settles, however, on a conclusion for the essay evokingresilienceandhopein thehuman conditionby describing a circumstance where thebaseballplayerRick Ankielreset his career back to the minor leagues as an outfielder after getting the yips as a major league pitcher.

See also

edit

References

edit
  1. ^Öhman, A. (2000). "Fear and anxiety: Evolutionary, cognitive, and clinical perspectives". In M. Lewis & J.M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.).Handbook of emotions.pp. 573–93. New York: The Guilford Press.
  2. ^abOlsson A, Phelps EA (September 2007). "Social learning of fear".Nature Neuroscience.10(9): 1095–102.doi:10.1038/nn1968.PMID17726475.S2CID11976458.
  3. ^Gill, M.J. and Burrow, R., 2017. The function of fear in institutional maintenance: Feeling frightened as an essential ingredient in haute cuisine.Organization Studies
  4. ^Edmundson LD."The Neurobiology of Fear".Serendip. Archived fromthe originalon 23 June 2017.Retrieved9 April2012.
  5. ^"Symptoms-phobias".nhs.uk/.27 July 2022.Archivedfrom the original on 20 October 2022.Retrieved30 March2023.
  6. ^Gray JA (1987).The Psychology of Fear and Stress(2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  7. ^abAdolphs R (January 2013)."The biology of fear".Current Biology.23(2): R79–93.Bibcode:2013CBio...23..R79A.doi:10.1016/j.cub.2012.11.055.PMC3595162.PMID23347946.
  8. ^abcdArcher J (1976). "The organization of aggression and fear in vertebrates". In Bateson PP, Klopfer PH (eds.).Perspectives in Ethology (Vol. 2).New York: Plenum. pp. 231–298.
  9. ^Russell PA (1976). "Fear-evoking stimuli". In Sluckin W (ed.).Fear in Animals and Man.Wokingham, UK: Van Nostrand Reinhold. pp. 86–124.
  10. ^Garcia R (September 2017)."Neurobiology of fear and specific phobias".Learning & Memory.24(9): 462–471.doi:10.1101/lm.044115.116.PMC5580526.PMID28814472.
  11. ^Ohman A, Mineka S (July 2001). "Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning".Psychological Review.108(3): 483–522.doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.483.PMID11488376.S2CID7920871.
  12. ^Bracha HS (July 2006)."Human brain evolution and the" Neuroevolutionary Time-depth Principle: "Implications for the Reclassification of fear-circuitry-related traits in DSM-V and for studying resilience to warzone-related posttraumatic stress disorder"(PDF).Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry.30(5): 827–853.doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2006.01.008.PMC7130737.PMID16563589.
  13. ^Olsson A, Nearing KI, Phelps EA (March 2007)."Learning fears by observing others: the neural systems of social fear transmission".Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience.2(1): 3–11.doi:10.1093/scan/nsm005.PMC2555428.PMID18985115.
  14. ^"Polio: MedlinePlus Medical Encyclopedia".Archivedfrom the original on 2017-01-29.Retrieved2017-01-25.
  15. ^Kim K, Markman AB (3 May 2005). "Differences in Fear of Isolation as an explanation of Cultural Differences: Evidence from memory and reasoning".Journal of Experimental Social Psychology.42(3): 350–364.doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2005.06.005.
  16. ^Warr M; Stafford M (1983). "Fear of Victimization: A Look at the Proximate Causes".Social Forces.61(4): 1033–1043.doi:10.1093/sf/61.4.1033.
  17. ^Welch A (October 15, 2015)."Things Americans fear most".CBS News.Archivedfrom the original on June 22, 2016.
  18. ^Ingraham C (October 30, 2014)."America's top fears: Public speaking, heights and bugs".The Washington Post.Archivedfrom the original on October 5, 2016.
  19. ^Brewer G (March 19, 2001)."Snakes Top List of American's Fears".Gallup.Archivedfrom the original on July 14, 2016.
  20. ^Gallup Poll: What Frightens America's YouthArchived2008-11-21 at theWayback Machine,gallup (29 March 2005).
  21. ^Tancer B (2008).Click: What millions of people are doing online and why it matters.Hyperion.ISBN978-1-4013-2304-2.
  22. ^Zerwekh J (2013).Illustrated Study Guide for the NCLEX-RN® Exam(8th ed.). Mosby, Inc. pp. 178–179.ISBN978-0-323-08232-7.Retrieved7 July2020.
  23. ^Barrettn LF, Lewis M, Haviland-Jones JM (2016).Handbook of Emotions.Guilford Publications. pp. 751–773.ISBN978-1-4625-2534-8.Archivedfrom the original on 2017-03-02.
  24. ^Burton LD (2011). "Fear".Journal of Research on Christian Education.20(2): 113–116.doi:10.1080/10656219.2011.592801.S2CID216092318.
  25. ^Fox ER (June 1987)."Fear of the unknown".The Western Journal of Medicine.146(6): 759–760.doi:10.1108/17578043200800026.PMC1307488.PMID18750277.S2CID72767139.
  26. ^Carleton RN (June 2016)."Fear of the unknown: One fear to rule them all?".Journal of Anxiety Disorders.41(June2016): 5–21.doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.03.011.PMID27067453.
  27. ^abcMisslin R (April 2003). "The defense system of fear: behavior and neurocircuitry".Neurophysiologie Clinique = Clinical Neurophysiology.33(2): 55–66.doi:10.1016/s0987-7053(03)00009-1.PMID12837573.S2CID35133426.
  28. ^Blanchard RJ, Blanchard DC, Rodgers J, Weiss SM (1990). "The characterization and modelling of antipredator defensive behavior".Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews.14(4): 463–472.doi:10.1016/s0149-7634(05)80069-7.PMID2287483.S2CID10132051.
  29. ^De Boer SF, Koolhaas JM (February 2003). "Defensive burying in rodents: ethology, neurobiology and psychopharmacology".European Journal of Pharmacology.463(1–3): 145–161.doi:10.1016/s0014-2999(03)01278-0.PMID12600707.
  30. ^abcArcher J (1979). "Behavioural aspects of fear". In Sluckin W (ed.).Fear in Animals and Man.Workingham, UK: Van Nostrand Reinhold. pp. 56–85.
  31. ^Stankowich T, Blumstein DT (December 2005)."Fear in animals: a meta-analysis and review of risk assessment".Proceedings. Biological Sciences.272(1581): 2627–2634.doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3251.PMC1559976.PMID16321785.
  32. ^Osborn JW, Liard JF, Cowley AW (April 1987). "The Economics of Fleeing from Predators".Effect of AVP on pressor responses to peripheral sympathetic stimulation in the rat.Advances in the Study of Behavior. Vol. 252. pp. H675–H680.doi:10.1016/S0065-3454(08)60192-8.ISBN9780120045167.PMID3565586.
  33. ^Morgan MA, LeDoux JE (August 1995). "Differential contribution of dorsal and ventral medial prefrontal cortex to the acquisition and extinction of conditioned fear in rats".Behavioral Neuroscience.109(4): 681–688.doi:10.1037/0735-7044.109.4.681.PMID7576212.S2CID3167606.
  34. ^Cammarota M, Bevilaqua LR, Kerr D, Medina JH, Izquierdo I (February 2003)."Inhibition of mRNA and protein synthesis in the CA1 region of the dorsal hippocampus blocks reinstallment of an extinguished conditioned fear response".The Journal of Neuroscience.23(3): 737–741.doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-03-00737.2003.PMC6741935.PMID12574401.
  35. ^Davis S (2008).21st Century Psychology: A Reference Handbook, Vol. 1.Thousand Oaks, California: Sage Publications, Inc. pp. 282–286.ISBN978-1-4129-4968-2.
  36. ^Robert P."The Amygdala and Its Allies".2002.The Brain.Archivedfrom the original on 6 August 2013.Retrieved2 October2013.
  37. ^Schmidt NB, Richey JA, Zvolensky MJ, Maner JK (September 2008)."Exploring human freeze responses to a threat stressor".Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry.39(3): 292–304.doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.08.002.PMC2489204.PMID17880916.
  38. ^Bracha HS (September 2004)."Freeze, flight, fight, fright, faint: adaptationist perspectives on the acute stress response spectrum"(PDF).CNS Spectrums.9(9): 679–685.doi:10.1017/s1092852900001954.PMID15337864.S2CID8430710.
  39. ^Adolphs R, Gosselin F, Buchanan TW, Tranel D, Schyns P, Damasio AR (January 2005)."A mechanism for impaired fear recognition after amygdala damage".Nature.433(7021): 68–72.Bibcode:2005Natur.433...68A.doi:10.1038/nature03086.PMID15635411.S2CID2139996.
  40. ^Bolles R (1970). "Species-Specific Defense Reactions and Avoidance Learning".Psychological Review.77(1): 32–48.doi:10.1037/h0028589.
  41. ^Crawford M, Masterson FA (1982)."Species-specific defense reactions and avoidance learning. An evaluative review".The Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science.17(4): 204–214.doi:10.1007/BF03001275.PMID6891452.S2CID142436039.
  42. ^Kiein S (2002).Biological Influences on Learning.Mississippi State University: McGraw-Hill Higher Education.Archivedfrom the original on 2008-12-05.
  43. ^Fanselow M(1986). "Associative vs topographical accounts of the immediate shock-freezing deficit in rats: Implications for the response selection rules governing species-specific defensive reactions".Learning and Motivation.17(1): 16–39.doi:10.1016/0023-9690(86)90018-4.
  44. ^Crawford M, Masterson FA (October 1982). "Species-specific defense reactions and avoidance learning".The Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science.17(4). Springer Science and Business Media LLC: 204–214.doi:10.1007/bf03001275.ISSN0093-2213.PMID6891452.S2CID142436039.
  45. ^Brocke B, Lesch KP, Armbruster D, Moser DA, Müller A, Strobel A, Kirschbaum C (January 2010). "Stathmin, a gene regulating neural plasticity, affects fear and anxiety processing in humans".American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B, Neuropsychiatric Genetics.153B(1): 243–251.doi:10.1002/ajmg.b.30989.PMID19526456.S2CID14851460.
  46. ^Kim JH, Ganella DE (2015-02-01). "A Review of Preclinical Studies to Understand Fear During Adolescence".Australian Psychologist.50(1): 25–31.doi:10.1111/ap.12066.ISSN1742-9544.S2CID142760996.
  47. ^Kim JH, Richardson R (February 2010). "New findings on extinction of conditioned fear early in development: theoretical and clinical implications".Biological Psychiatry.67(4): 297–303.doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.09.003.PMID19846065.S2CID33444381.
  48. ^Li S, Kim JH, Richardson R (April 2012). "Differential involvement of the medial prefrontal cortex in the expression of learned fear across development".Behavioral Neuroscience.126(2): 217–225.doi:10.1037/a0027151.PMID22448855.
  49. ^Scheller E, Büchel C, Gamer M (2012-07-25)."Diagnostic features of emotional expressions are processed preferentially".PLOS ONE.7(7): e41792.Bibcode:2012PLoSO...741792S.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0041792.PMC3405011.PMID22848607.
  50. ^Smith ML, Cottrell GW, Gosselin F, Schyns PG (March 2005). "Transmitting and decoding facial expressions".Psychological Science.16(3): 184–189.doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00801.x.PMID15733197.S2CID2622673.
  51. ^Elsherif MM, Sahan MI, Rotshtein P (2017-03-07)."The perceptual saliency of fearful eyes and smiles: A signal detection study".PLOS ONE.12(3): e0173199.Bibcode:2017PLoSO..1273199E.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0173199.PMC5340363.PMID28267761.
  52. ^Best, Ben (2004).The Amygdala and the EmotionsArchived2007-03-09 at theWayback Machine.benbest
  53. ^Gleitman, Henry; Fridlund, Alan J. and Reisberg, Daniel (2004).Psychology(6th ed.). W.W. Norton & Company.ISBN0-393-97767-6.
  54. ^abTravis J (2004). "Fear not: Scientists are learning how people can unlearn fear".Science News.165(3): 42–44.doi:10.2307/4014925.JSTOR4014925.
  55. ^von Bohlen und Halbach, O; Dermietzel, R (2006).Neurotransmitters and neuromodulators: handbook of receptors and biological effects.Wiley-VCH. p. 125.ISBN978-3-527-31307-5.
  56. ^Hoehn K, Marieb EN (2010).Human Anatomy & Physiology.San Francisco: Benjamin Cummings.ISBN0-321-60261-7.
  57. ^Amunts K, Kedo O, Kindler M, Pieperhoff P, Mohlberg H, Shah NJ, et al. (December 2005). "Cytoarchitectonic mapping of the human amygdala, hippocampal region and entorhinal cortex: intersubject variability and probability maps".Anatomy and Embryology.210(5–6): 343–352.doi:10.1007/s00429-005-0025-5.PMID16208455.S2CID6984617.
  58. ^Schacter, Daniel L.; Gilbert, Daniel T. and Wegner, Daniel M. (2011)Psychology Study Guide,Worth Publishers,ISBN1-4292-0615-2.
  59. ^LeDoux J (October 2003). "The emotional brain, fear, and the amygdala".Cellular and Molecular Neurobiology.23(4–5): 727–738.doi:10.1023/A:1025048802629.PMID14514027.S2CID3216382.
  60. ^American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV. Washington, DC.ISBN0-89042-061-0.
  61. ^Cheng DT, Knight DC, Smith CN, Stein EA, Helmstetter FJ (February 2003)."Functional MRI of human amygdala activity during Pavlovian fear conditioning: stimulus processing versus response expression"(PDF).Behavioral Neuroscience.117(1): 3–10.CiteSeerX10.1.1.123.4317.doi:10.1037/0735-7044.117.1.3.PMID12619902.Archived fromthe original(PDF)on 2008-10-08.Retrieved2017-10-24.
  62. ^Berdoy M, Webster JP, Macdonald DW (August 2000)."Fatal attraction in rats infected with Toxoplasma gondii".Proceedings. Biological Sciences.267(1452): 1591–1594.doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1182.PMC1690701.PMID11007336.
  63. ^Larkin M (1997). "Amygdala differentiates fear response".The Lancet.350(9073): 268.doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(05)62234-9.S2CID54232230.
  64. ^abRadua J, Phillips ML, Russell T, Lawrence N, Marshall N, Kalidindi S, et al. (January 2010)."Neural response to specific components of fearful faces in healthy and schizophrenic adults".NeuroImage.49(1): 939–946.doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.030.PMID19699306.S2CID6209163.Archivedfrom the original on 2017-12-01.Retrieved2019-08-14.
  65. ^Fear not. "Ski Mar.–Apr. 2009: 15. Gale Canada In Context. Web. 29 Sep. 2011
  66. ^Moser JC, Brownlee RC, Silverstein R (April 1968). "Alarm pheromones of the ant atta texana".Journal of Insect Physiology.14(4): 529–535.Bibcode:1968JInsP..14..529M.doi:10.1016/0022-1910(68)90068-1.PMID5649232.
  67. ^Ressler RH, Cialdini RB, Ghoca ML, Kleist SM (August 1968). "Alarm pheromone in the earthworm Lumbricus terrestris".Science.161(3841): 597–599.Bibcode:1968Sci...161..597R.doi:10.1126/science.161.3841.597.PMID5663305.S2CID30927186.
  68. ^Rottman SJ, Snowdon CT (December 1972). "Demonstration and analysis of an alarm pheromone in mice".Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology.81(3): 483–490.doi:10.1037/h0033703.PMID4649187.
  69. ^Fanselow MS (June 1985). "Odors released by stressed rats produce opioid analgesia in unstressed rats".Behavioral Neuroscience.99(3): 589–592.doi:10.1037/0735-7044.99.3.589.PMID3843728.
  70. ^Núñez J, Almeida L, Balderrama N, Giurfa M (December 1997). "Alarm pheromone induces stress analgesia via an opioid system in the honeybee".Physiology & Behavior.63(1): 75–80.doi:10.1016/s0031-9384(97)00391-0.PMID9402618.S2CID8788442.
  71. ^Abel EL, Bilitzke PJ (August 1990). "A possible alarm substance in the forced swimming test".Physiology & Behavior.48(2): 233–239.doi:10.1016/0031-9384(90)90306-o.PMID2255725.S2CID22994036.
  72. ^Abel EL (October 1991). "Alarm substance emitted by rats in the forced-swim test is a low volatile pheromone".Physiology & Behavior.50(4): 723–727.doi:10.1016/0031-9384(91)90009-d.PMID1775546.S2CID41044786.
  73. ^Cocke R, Moynihan JA, Cohen N, Grota LJ, Ader R (March 1993)."Exposure to conspecific alarm chemosignals alters immune responses in BALB/c mice".Brain, Behavior, and Immunity.7(1): 36–46.doi:10.1006/brbi.1993.1004.PMID8471798.S2CID7196539.
  74. ^Abel EL (June 1994). "The pituitary mediates production or release of an alarm chemosignal in rats".Hormones and Behavior.28(2): 139–145.doi:10.1006/hbeh.1994.1011.PMID7927280.S2CID11844089.
  75. ^Kiyokawa Y, Kikusui T, Takeuchi Y, Mori Y (January 2004)."Alarm pheromones with different functions are released from different regions of the body surface of male rats".Chemical Senses.29(1): 35–40.doi:10.1093/chemse/bjh004.PMID14752038.
  76. ^Kiyokawa Y, Shimozuru M, Kikusui T, Takeuchi Y, Mori Y (February 2006)."Alarm pheromone increases defensive and risk assessment behaviors in male rats".Physiology & Behavior.87(2): 383–387.doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.11.003.PMID16337975.S2CID12780994.Archivedfrom the original on 2017-08-30.Retrieved2017-08-30.
  77. ^Arakawa H, Arakawa K, Blandino P, Deak T (May 2011). "The role of neuroinflammation in the release of aversive odor cues from footshock-stressed rats: Implications for the neural mechanism of alarm pheromone".Psychoneuroendocrinology.36(4): 557–568.doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.09.001.PMID20888127.S2CID24367179.
  78. ^Kiyokawa Y, Kikusui T, Takeuchi Y, Mori Y (May 2005)."Mapping the neural circuit activated by alarm pheromone perception by c-Fos immunohistochemistry".Brain Research.1043(1–2): 145–154.doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2005.02.061.PMID15862528.S2CID41186952.Archivedfrom the original on 2017-08-31.Retrieved2017-08-31.
  79. ^Inagaki H, Kiyokawa Y, Takeuchi Y, Mori Y (February 2010). "The alarm pheromone in male rats as a unique anxiety model: psychopharmacological evidence using anxiolytics".Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior.94(4): 575–579.doi:10.1016/j.pbb.2009.11.013.PMID19969015.S2CID28194770.
  80. ^Zou J, Storm DR, Xia Z (2013)."Conditional deletion of ERK5 MAP kinase in the nervous system impairs pheromone information processing and pheromone-evoked behaviors".PLOS ONE.8(10): e76901.Bibcode:2013PLoSO...876901Z.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0076901.PMC3793934.PMID24130808.
  81. ^Cohen S, Wills TA (September 1985). "Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis".Psychological Bulletin.98(2): 310–357.doi:10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.310.PMID3901065.S2CID18137066.
  82. ^Takahashi Y, Kiyokawa Y, Kodama Y, Arata S, Takeuchi Y, Mori Y (March 2013)."Olfactory signals mediate social buffering of conditioned fear responses in male rats".Behavioural Brain Research.240:46–51.doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2012.11.017.PMID23183219.S2CID30938917.Archivedfrom the original on 2017-08-31.Retrieved2017-08-31.
  83. ^Rittschof CC, Robinson GE (November 2013)."Manipulation of colony environment modulates honey bee aggression and brain gene expression".Genes, Brain and Behavior.12(8): 802–811.doi:10.1111/gbb.12087.PMC3863782.PMID24034579.
  84. ^Ferrer RP, Zimmer RK (October 2012). "Community ecology and the evolution of molecules of keystone significance".The Biological Bulletin.223(2): 167–177.doi:10.1086/BBLv223n2p167.PMID23111129.S2CID592393.
  85. ^Lübke KT, Pause BM (January 2014). "Sex-hormone dependent perception of androstenone suggests its involvement in communicating competition and aggression".Physiology & Behavior.123:136–141.doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.10.016.PMID24184511.S2CID25729942.
  86. ^Prehn A, Ohrt A, Sojka B, Ferstl R, Pause BM (February 2006). "Chemosensory anxiety signals augment the startle reflex in humans".Neuroscience Letters.394(2): 127–130.doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2005.10.012.PMID16257486.S2CID23295966.
  87. ^Prehn-Kristensen A, Wiesner C, Bergmann TO, Wolff S, Jansen O, Mehdorn HM, et al. (June 2009)."Induction of empathy by the smell of anxiety".PLOS ONE.4(6): e5987.Bibcode:2009PLoSO...4.5987P.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0005987.PMC2695008.PMID19551135.
  88. ^Radulescu AR, Mujica-Parodi LR (Jul 2013)."Human gender differences in the perception of conspecific alarm chemosensory cues".PLOS ONE.8(7): e68485.Bibcode:2013PLoSO...868485R.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0068485.PMC3722227.PMID23894310.
  89. ^Frey MC, Weyers P, Pauli P, Mühlberger A (June 2012). "Androstadienone in motor reactions of men and women toward angry faces".Perceptual and Motor Skills.114(3): 807–825.doi:10.2466/07.16.22.28.PMS.114.3.807-825.PMID22913022.S2CID13194791.
  90. ^abcvan Kampen HS (February 2019). "The principle of consistency and the cause and function of behaviour".Behavioural Processes.159:42–54.doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2018.12.013.PMID30562561.S2CID56478466.
  91. ^Hebb DO (September 1946). "On the nature of fear".Psychological Review.53(5): 259–276.doi:10.1037/h0061690.PMID20285975.S2CID5211697.
  92. ^Raber J, Arzy S, Bertolus JB, Depue B, Haas HE, Hofmann SG, et al. (October 2019)."Current understanding of fear learning and memory in humans and animal models and the value of a linguistic approach for analyzing fear learning and memory in humans".Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews.105:136–177.doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.03.015.PMID30970272.S2CID102353770.
  93. ^Hartley CA, Phelps EA (2013). "Fear Models in Animals and Humans". In Vasa RA, Roy AK (eds.).Pediatric Anxiety Disorders.New York: Springer New York. pp. 3–21.doi:10.1007/978-1-4614-6599-7_1.ISBN978-1-4614-6598-0.
  94. ^Luchkina NV, Bolshakov VY (January 2019)."Mechanisms of fear learning and extinction: synaptic plasticity-fear memory connection".Psychopharmacology.236(1): 163–182.doi:10.1007/s00213-018-5104-4.PMC6374177.PMID30415278.
  95. ^Maren S (2001)."Neurobiology of Pavlovian fear conditioning".Annual Review of Neuroscience.24(1): 897–931.doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.897.hdl:2027.42/61939.PMID11520922.
  96. ^abcdeRichter-Levin G, Stork O, Schmidt MV (August 2019)."Animal models of PTSD: a challenge to be met".Molecular Psychiatry.24(8): 1135–1156.doi:10.1038/s41380-018-0272-5.PMC6756084.PMID30816289.
  97. ^Zoladz PR, Eisenmann ED, Rose RM, Kohls BA, Johnson BL, Robinson KL, et al. (August 2018)."Predator-based psychosocial stress model of PTSD differentially influences voluntary ethanol consumption depending on methodology".Alcohol.70:33–41.doi:10.1016/j.alcohol.2018.01.004.PMID29775837.S2CID21734755.
  98. ^Willner P (February 2017)."The chronic mild stress (CMS) model of depression: History, evaluation and usage".Neurobiology of Stress.6:78–93.doi:10.1016/j.ynstr.2016.08.002.PMC5314424.PMID28229111.
  99. ^Abdallah CG, Geha P (February 2017)."Chronic Pain and Chronic Stress: Two Sides of the Same Coin?".Chronic Stress.1.doi:10.1177/2470547017704763.PMC5546756.PMID28795169.
  100. ^Lisieski MJ, Eagle AL, Conti AC, Liberzon I, Perrine SA (2018-05-15)."Single-Prolonged Stress: A Review of Two Decades of Progress in a Rodent Model of Post-traumatic Stress Disorder".Frontiers in Psychiatry.9:196.doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00196.PMC5962709.PMID29867615.
  101. ^abSouza RR, Noble LJ, McIntyre CK (2017-09-11)."Using the Single Prolonged Stress Model to Examine the Pathophysiology of PTSD".Frontiers in Pharmacology.8:615.doi:10.3389/fphar.2017.00615.PMC5600994.PMID28955225.
  102. ^Rau V, DeCola JP, Fanselow MS (January 2005)."Stress-induced enhancement of fear learning: an animal model of posttraumatic stress disorder".Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews.29(8): 1207–1223.doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.04.010.PMID16095698.S2CID5896548.
  103. ^Rajbhandari AK, Gonzalez ST, Fanselow MS (October 2018)."Stress-Enhanced Fear Learning, a Robust Rodent Model of Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder".Journal of Visualized Experiments(140): 58306.doi:10.3791/58306.PMC6235522.PMID30371665.
  104. ^Rau V, Fanselow MS (March 2009)."Exposure to a stressor produces a long lasting enhancement of fear learning in rats".Stress.12(2): 125–133.doi:10.1080/10253890802137320.PMID18609302.S2CID15453890.
  105. ^Sandi C (April 2011)."Healing anxiety disorders with glucocorticoids".Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.108(16): 6343–6344.Bibcode:2011PNAS..108.6343S.doi:10.1073/pnas.1103410108.PMC3080972.PMID21482789.
  106. ^Kolber BJ, Roberts MS, Howell MP, Wozniak DF, Sands MS, Muglia LJ (August 2008)."Central amygdala glucocorticoid receptor action promotes fear-associated CRH activation and conditioning".Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.105(33): 12004–12009.Bibcode:2008PNAS..10512004K.doi:10.1073/pnas.0803216105.PMC2575312.PMID18695245.
  107. ^abKaplan JS, Tolin DF (2011). "Exposure therapy for anxiety disorders: Theoretical mechanisms of exposure and treatment strategies".Psychiatric Times.28(9): 33–37.ProQuest894207776.
  108. ^"Cure Your Fear – Phobia Treatment".FearOf.net.Retrieved2018-11-28.
  109. ^"World With No Fear".NPR.2015-01-16.Archivedfrom the original on 2015-01-27.Retrieved2015-01-27.
  110. ^Goldenberg JL, Pyszczynski T, Greenberg J, Solomon S (August 2000). "Fleeing the Body: A Terror Management Perspective on the Problem of Human Corporeality".Personality and Social Psychology Review.4(3): 200–218.doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0403_1.ISSN1088-8683.S2CID31331978.
  111. ^abcFry PS (September 2003). "Perceived self-efficacy domains as predictors of fear of the unknown and fear of dying among older adults".Psychology and Aging.18(3): 474–486.doi:10.1037/0882-7974.18.3.474.PMID14518809.
  112. ^abKagan, Shelly.Lecture 22: Fear of DeathArchived2012-03-09 at theWayback MachineinPHIL 176: DeathArchived2017-06-09 at theWayback Machine.Yale Open Course 2007.
  113. ^Kahoe RD, Dunn RF (1976). "The fear of death and religious attitudes and behavior".Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion.14(4): 379–382.doi:10.2307/1384409.JSTOR1384409.
  114. ^Bassett JF, Bussard ML (February 2021). "Examining the Complex Relation Among Religion, Morality, and Death Anxiety: Religion Can Be a Source of Comfort and Concern Regarding Fears of Death".Omega.82(3): 467–487.doi:10.1177/0030222818819343.PMID30572785.S2CID58619649.
  115. ^Wink P (2006). "Who is afraid of death? Religiousness, spirituality, and death anxiety in late adulthood".Journal of Religion, Spirituality & Aging.18(2): 93–110.doi:10.1300/J496v18n02_08.S2CID144684731.
  116. ^ Minnis, Alastair(2012). "Other Worlds: Chaucer's Classicism". In Copeland, Rita (ed.).The Oxford History of Classical Reception in English Literature.Vol. 1: 800-1558. Oxford: Oxford University Press. p. 426.ISBN9780199587230.Retrieved4 November2023.[...] the remark 'Primus in orbe deos fecit timor' ('fear first made gods in the world') is uttered byCapaneus,a madman and blasphemer [...].
  117. ^Strimple RB."The Fear of the Lord".The Orthodox Presbyterian Church.
  118. ^"TAQWA: Fearing Allah".AHYA.ORG - Authentic Islamic Resources and Information.Archived fromthe originalon 2016-03-04.Retrieved2015-08-03.
  119. ^"Taḳwā",Encyclopaedia of Islam(2012).
  120. ^"Fear of God".Jewishvirtuallibrary.org.Retrieved12 January2015.
  121. ^"Fear and Manipulation: Perfect Together".psychopathsandlove.November 2014.Retrieved2018-11-27.
  122. ^Lupia, Arthur; Menning, Jesse O. (January 2009)."When Can Politicians Scare Citizens Into Supporting Bad Policies?".American Journal of Political Science.53(1): 90–106.doi:10.1111/j.1540-5907.2008.00359.x.ISSN0092-5853.S2CID13496294.
  123. ^"Dystopia facts, information, pictures".encyclopedia.Archivedfrom the original on 4 March 2017.Retrieved3 March2017.
  124. ^Kyle RG (2012-08-01).Apocalyptic Fever: End-Time Prophecies in Modern America.Wipf and Stock Publishers.ISBN978-1-62189-410-0.Archivedfrom the original on 25 December 2017.Retrieved3 March2017.
  125. ^Yeung K, Zhang Z (24 January 2014)."The Neverending Apocalypse".The Princeton Buffer.Archivedfrom the original on 4 March 2017.Retrieved3 March2017.
  126. ^Schmidt C (19 November 2014)."Why are Dystopian Films on the Rise Again?".Archivedfrom the original on 4 March 2017.Retrieved3 March2017.
  127. ^Meszaros, E. L. (2020-09-23)."The Magnus Archives: A Beginner's Guide to the Popular Horror Podcast".CBR.Archivedfrom the original on 1 November 2020.
  128. ^"The Magnus Archives Episode 80 Transcript: The Librarian".The Magnus Archives Transcripts Archive Archive A.
  129. ^"The Magnus Archives Episode 35 Transcript: Hive".The Magnus Archives Transcripts Archive Archive A.
  130. ^"The Magnus Archives Episode 102 Transcript: Nesting Instinct".The Magnus Archives Transcripts Archive Archive A.
  131. ^abcConroy DE, Poczwardowski A, Henschen KP (2001). "Evaluative criteria and consequences associated with failure and success for elite athletes and performing artists".Journal of Applied Sport Psychology.13(3): 300–322.doi:10.1080/104132001753144428.S2CID146422220.
  132. ^abcdLazarus RS (1991).Emotion and Adaptation.New York: Oxford University Press.
  133. ^Birney RC, Burdick H, Teevan RC (1969).Fear of failure.Van Nostrand-Reinhold Company.
  134. ^Duda JL (1993). "Goals: A social-cognitive approach to the study of achievement motivation in sport.". In Singer RN, Murphey M, Tennant LK (eds.).Handbook of research on sport psychology.New York: Macmillan. pp. 421–436.
  135. ^Murray H (1938).Explorations in Personal.New York: Oxford University Press.
  136. ^abcConroy DE, Elliot AJ (2004). "Fear of failure and achievement goals in sport: Addressing the issue of the chicken and the egg".Anxiety, Stress & Coping.17(3): 271–285.CiteSeerX10.1.1.643.3752.doi:10.1080/1061580042000191642.S2CID15144896.
  137. ^Green, John (2021).The Anthropocene Reviewed: Essays on a Human-Centered Planet.[New York, New York]: Dutton Penguin.ISBN978-0525555216.

Further reading

edit
edit