Cetacean surfacing behaviouris a grouping of movement types thatcetaceansmake at the water's surface in addition to breathing. Cetaceans have developed and use surface behaviours for many functions such as display, feeding and communication. All regularly observed members of the order Cetacea, including whales, dolphins and porpoises, show a range of surfacing behaviours.
Cetacea is usually split into two suborders,OdontocetiandMysticeti,based on the presence of teeth or baleen plates in adults respectively. However, when considering behaviour, Cetacea can be split into whales (cetaceans more than 10 m long such as sperm and most baleen whales) and dolphins and porpoises (all Odontocetes less than 10 m long includingorca[1]) as many behaviours are correlated with size.
Although some behaviours such as spyhopping, logging and lobtailing occur in both groups, others such as bow riding or peduncle throws are exclusive to one or the other. It is these energetic behaviours that humans observe most frequently, which has resulted in a large amount of scientific literature on the subject and a populartourism industry.
Travelling surface behaviour
editBreaching and lunging
edit-
Humpback whale spinner-breaching
-
Dwarf sperm whalebreaching
-
Hector's DolphinsoffCloudy Bay,New Zealand
Abreachor alungeis a leap out of the water, also known as cresting. The distinction between the two is fairly arbitrary: cetacean researcherHal Whiteheaddefines a breach as any leap in which at least 40% of the animal's body clears the water, and a lunge as a leap with less than 40% clearance.[2]Qualitatively, a breach is a genuine jump with an intent to clear the water, whereas a lunge is the result of a fast upward-sloping swim that has caused the whale to clear the surface of the water unintentionally. This latter "lunging" behaviour is often a result of feeding inrorquals.[3]The right, humpback, and sperm whales are the most widely observed jumpers. However otherbaleen whalessuch asfin,blue,minke,grayandseiwhales also breach.Oceanic dolphins,including theorca,are very common breachers and are in fact capable of lifting themselves completely out of the water very easily, although there is little distinction between this andporpoising.Some non-cetacean marine creatures also exhibit breaching behavior, such as several shark species and rays of the generaMantaandMobula.[4]
Two techniques are used by cetaceans in order to breach. The first method, most common in sperm and humpback whales, is conducted by swimming vertically upwards from depth, and heading straight out of the water.[5]The other more common method is to travel close to the surface and parallel to it, and then jerk upwards at full speed with as few as 3 tail strokes to perform a breach.[5][6]In all breaches the cetacean clears the water with the majority of its body at an acute angle, such as an average of 30° to the horizontal as recorded in sperm whales.[7]The whale then turns to land on its back or side, and less frequently may not turn but "belly flop" instead. In order to achieve 90% clearance, a humpback needs to leave the water at a speed of eight metres per second or 29 kilometres per hour (18 mph). For a 36 metric tons (40 short tons) animal, this results in amomentumof 288 thousandnewton seconds.Despite its energetic cost, breaching is often carried out in series. The longest recorded sustained series was by a humpback near theWest Indiestotaling 130 leaps in less than 90 minutes.[8]Repeated breaches tire the animal, so less of the body clears the water each time.[9]
Ultimately, the reasons for breaching are unknown; however, there is evidence to support a range of hypotheses. Whales are more likely to breach when they are in groups, suggesting that it is a non-verbal signal to other group members during social behaviour. Scientists have called this theory"honest signalling".The immense cloud of bubbles and underwater disturbance following a breach cannot be faked; neighbours then know a breach has taken place. A single breach costs a whale only about 0.075% of its total daily energy intake, but a long series of breaches may add up to a significant energy expenditure.[9]A breach is therefore a sign that the animal is physically fit enough to afford energy for this acrobatic display, hence it could be used for ascertaining dominance, courting or warning of danger.[5]It is also possible that the loud "smack" upon re-entering is useful for stunning or scaring prey, similar tolobtailing.As breaching is often seen in rough seas it is possible that a breach allows the whale to breathe in air that is not close to the surface and full of spray, or that they use breaching to communicate when the noise of the ocean would mask acoustic signals.[10]Another widely accepted possible reason is to dislodge parasites from the skin.[10]The behaviour may also be more simply a form of play.[10]
Porpoising
edit-
Southern right whale dolphinsporpoising
Porpoising,also known as running,[11]is a high speed surface behaviour of small cetaceans where long jumps are alternated with swimming close to the surface. Despite the name, porpoising behaviour is seen in dolphins and porpoises, as well as other marine species such aspenguins[12]andpinnipeds.[13]Whenmarine mammalsare travelling at speed they are forced to stay close to the surface in order to maintainrespirationfor the energetic exercise. At leisurely cruising speeds below 4.6 m/s, dolphins swim below the water's surface and only briefly expose theirblowholesalong with up to one third of their body at any one time.[11]This results in little splashing as they have a very streamlined shape.[13]Porpoising occurs mainly when dolphins and porpoises are swimming at speeds greater than 4.6 m/s.[11]Here, jump length is roughly equal to distance traveled when the cetaceans are submerged.[11]This exposes the blowhole for longer which is needed to get enough oxygen to maintainmetabolismand therefore high speeds over long periods of time. Studies have also shown that leaping is more energetically efficient than swimming above a certain threshold speed.[11]This is due to the reduction in friction when travelling in air compared to water which saves more energy than is needed to produce the leap.[13]These benefits also outweigh the energy wasted due to the large amount of splashing often seen when groups are porpoising.[11]Porpoising is therefore a result of high speed swimming which cetaceans use for important pursuit and escape activities. For example, dolphins may be seen porpoising away from their main predator,sharks[14]or the direction of incoming boats to avoid collision.[15]
Although porpoising is a useful product of rapid swimming, much variation seen in the behaviour cannot be explained by this cause alone; it has likely evolved to provide other functions. For example, the rotation during porpoising by thespinner dolphinleads to much splashing and is more common at slower speeds[11]so cannot be attributed to an energy saving mechanism. It is therefore more likely to be a form of play or communication within or between pods.[11]Another reason might be to removebarnaclesorremorasthat, when attached, increase drag during swimming.[16]When spinner dolphins impact the water the combination ofcentrifugaland vertical force upon theseectoparasitescan be up to 700 times their own weight and so efficiently remove them.[16]Other theories suggest that cetaceans may porpoise in order to observe distant objects such as food by looking for visual cues, such as birds dive-bombing abait ball.[17]Research into the additional functions of porpoising has so far been focussed on the more acrobatic species, but it is likely that other cetaceans also use it for these, and perhaps unknown, reasons too.
Wave or bow-riding and following vessels
edit-
"Type B" orcas offSouth Georgia
-
Belugasfollowing watching boats inChurchill River
The termwave-ridingis most commonly used to describe the surface activity of cetaceans that approach boats and jump repeatedly in the waves produced by the boats. This includesbow-riding,where cetaceans are in the pressure wave in front of the boat, andwake-riding,where they are off the stern in the wake.[18]Cetaceans swim using fluke propulsion when experiencing wave energy below the threshold needed for riding, such as when boats travel at speeds slower than 3 m/s[19]or when they are outside of the peak wave energy zone. However, at higher speeds dolphins and porpoises will seek out the pressure wave and its maximum energy zone in order to ride the wave by holding their flukes in a fixed plane, with only minor adjustments for repositioning.[19]Wave-riding reduces the energetic cost of swimming to the dolphin, even when compared to slower swimming speeds.[19]For example,heart rate,metabolic rateand transport cost was reduced by up to 70% during wave-riding compared to swimming at speeds 1 m/s slower inbottlenose dolphin.[19]Wave-riding behaviour can be performed by dolphins from minutes up to several hours,[19]and therefore is a useful energy-saving mechanism for swimming at higher speeds.
Wave-riding is most common in smallOdontocetes.It has also been observed in larger cetaceans such asfalse killer whalesandorca,[20][21]although most larger Odontocetes do not seek out any form of interaction with boats. Bow-riding is the most common form of interactive behaviour with boats across a variety of smaller Odontocete species, such as dolphins in the generaStenellaandDelphinus.[22]The type of interaction can often depend on the behavioral state of the group as well as species. For example, spotted dolphins are more likely to interact when travelling or milling but less likely when they are socialising or surface feeding.[22]Interactive behavior may also depend on group composition, as both orca and bottlenose dolphins have been recorded to interact mostly when a calf was in the group.[22][23]This indicates that groups with calves may approach boats in order to teach the young how to interact safely to avoid collision. Another result of cetaceans traveling in pods is an increase in competition for the optimal wave energy and so maximum energy saving position. Position of individuals may reflect the dominancehierarchyof the pod and therefore could be used to ascertain dominance.[21]Severalrorquals,such asminke,[24]sei,[25]bryde's,[26]humpback,[27]andgray[28]are also known to display actions in similar manners.
Gallery
editStationary surface behaviour
editSpyhopping
edit-
Orca ( "type C" ) spyhopping
-
Southern minke whalespyhopping in Antarctica
Whenspyhopping,the whale rises and holds a vertical position partially out of the water, often exposing its entirerostrumand head. It is visually akin to a humantreading water.Spyhopping is controlled and slow, and can last for minutes at a time if the whale is sufficiently inquisitive about whatever it is viewing. Generally, the whale does not appear to swim by fluke propulsion to maintain its "elevated" position while spyhopping, instead relying on exceptional buoyancy control and positioning with pectoral fins. Typically the whale's eyes will be slightly above or below the surface of the water, enabling it to see whatever is nearby on the surface.[29]Different species of sharks, including thegreat white sharkandoceanic whitetip shark,have also been known to spyhop.[30][31]
Spyhopping often occurs during a "mugging" situation, where the focus of a whale's attention is on a boat, such as whale-watching tours, which they sometimes approach and interact with.[32]On the other hand, spyhopping amongorcasis thought to aid predation, as they are often seen around ice floes attempting to viewpreyspecies such as seals that are resting on the floes.[33]When prey is detected the individual will conduct a series of spy-hops from different locations around it, then vocalise to the group members to do the same to possibly prepare for an attack.[33]In this instance a spyhop may be more useful than a breach, because the view is held steady for a longer period of time. Often when cetaceans breach, their eyes do not clear the water, which suggests it might not be used for looking but instead for hearing. For example,gray whaleswill often spy-hop in order to hear better when they are near the line where waves begin to break in the ocean as this marks out their migration route.[29]
Lobtailing and slapping
edit-
Humpback whale lobtailing at Ísafjarðardjúp, Iceland
-
Humpback whale tail-slapping off the coast of Molokai, Hawaii
-
Humpback whale tail-slapping withCalifornia sea lions
-
Bowhead whale tail-slapping in Shantar Islands
Lobtailingis the act of a whale or dolphin lifting itsflukesout of the water and then bringing them down onto the surface of the water hard and fast in order to make a loud slap. Large whales tend to lobtail by positioning themselves vertically downwards into the water and then slapping the surface by bending the tail stock. Dolphins, however, tend to remain horizontal, either on their belly or their back, and make the slap via a jerky whole body movement. All species are likely to slap several times in a single session. Like breaching, lobtailing is common amongst active cetacean species such as sperm, humpback, right andgrey whales.It is less common, but still occasionally occurs, amongst the other large whales.Porpoisesandriver dolphinsrarely lobtail, but it is a very common phenomenon amongstoceanic dolphins.Lobtailing is more common within species that have a complex social order than those where animals are more likely to be solitary. Lobtailing often occurs in conjunction with other aerial behaviour such as breaching. Species with largeflippersmay also slap them against the water for a similar effect, known aspectoral slapping.[citation needed]
The sound of a lobtail can be heard underwater several hundred metres from the site of a slap. This has led to speculation amongst scientists that lobtailing is, like breaching, a form of non-vocal communication. However, studies ofbowhead whaleshave shown that the noise of a lobtail travels much less well than that of a vocal call or a breach. Thus the lobtail is probably important visually as well as acoustically, and may be a sign of aggression. Some suggest that lobtailing inhumpback whalesis a means of foraging. The hypothesis is that the loud noise causes fish to become frightened, thus tightening theirschooltogether, making it easier for the humpback to feed on them.[34]In this instance, lobtail feeding behaviour appeared to progressively spread throughout the population, as it increased from 0 to 50% of the population using it over the 9-year study.[34]As no individual under 2 years old nor any mothers were observed to use lobtail feeding it suggests that it is taught in foraging groups. The spread of lobtail feeding amongst humpback whales indicates its success as a novel foraging method.[34]
Gallery
edit- Whales with raised tailon Wikimedia Commons.
Peduncle throw
editApedunclethrow,also known aspeduncling,is a surfacing behaviour unique to humpback whales. During this the humpback converts its forward momentum into a crack-the-whip rotation, pivoting with its pectorals as it drives its head downward and thrusts its entire fluke and peduncle (the muscular rear portion of the torso) out of the water and sideways, before crashing into the water with terrific force. Peduncling takes place among the focal animals (female, escort, challenging male) in a competitive group, apparently as an aggressive gesture. Possibilities include escorts fending off a particular challenging male, females who seem agitated with an escort, or an individual not comfortable with a watching boat's presence. Occasionally, one whale performs a series of dozens of peduncle throws, directed at the same target each time.[35]
Pectoral slapping
edit-
Southern right whaleshowing pectoral fins
-
Humpback whales
Pectoral slapping,informally known as pec-slapping, is when a cetacean turns on its side, exposes one or bothpectoral finsinto the air, and then slaps them against the surface of the water. It is a form of non-vocal communication[36]commonly observed in a variety of whale and dolphin species as well as seals. The motion is slow and controlled, and the behaviour can occur repeatedly by one individual over a few minutes.[37]Thehumpback whale's pectoral fin is the largest appendage of any mammal and humpbacks are known for their extremely acrobatic behaviour. Pec-slapping varies between groups of different social structure, such as not occurring in lone males but being common in mother calf pairs and also when they are accompanied by an escort.[37]The reasons for pec-slapping therefore can vary depending on age and sex of individual humpback whales. During the breeding season adult males pec-slap before they disassociate with a group of males that are vying for a female, whereas adult females pec-slap to attract potential mates and indicate that she is sexually receptive.[38]Its function between mother calf pairs is less well known but is likely to be a form of play and communication that is taught to the calf by the mother for use when it is sexually mature.[38]Pectoral slapping has also been observed in theright whale,but due to its smaller size, the sound produced will be quieter[39]and therefore used for communication over smaller distances unlike the humpback. Exposure of the pectoral fin and consequent slapping has also been infrequently observed in blue whales, where it is most often a by-product of lunge feeding followed by rolling on to its side.
Logging
edit-
Southern right whales resting
-
Bowhead whale sleeping
-
Gray whalecavorting
Loggingis a behaviour that whales exhibit when at rest and appear like "logs" at the surface.[40]It is defined as lying without forward movement at the surface of the water with the dorsal fin or parts of the back are exposed.[41]Whales often rest for periods of time under the surface in order to sleep in mainly horizontal positions, although sperm whales also rest vertically.[42]However, as they consciously need to breathe at the surface, they can rest only one-half of their brain at a time, known asunihemispheric slow-wave sleep.This sleep pattern has been identified in all five cetacean species that have been tested for it thus far.[43]Cetaceans intermittently come to the surface in order to breathe during these sleep periods and exhibit logging behaviour. Logging can occur interchangeably with surface resting behaviour when cetaceans are travelling slowly, which is particularly common in mother-calf pairs,[44]as the young tire quickly during swimming. Logging is common, particularly inright whales,sperm whales,pilot whalesandhumpback whales.Another behaviour that may be mistaken for logging ismilling,where a group of cetaceans at the surface have little or no directional movement[45]but instead socialise with each other. This behaviour is particularly common in large groups of pilot whales.[45]
Dive times
editTime intervals between surfacing can vary depending on the species, surfacing style or the purpose of the dive; some species have been known to dive for up to 85 minutes at a time when hunting,[46]and dives in excess of three hours have been observed inCuvier's beaked whaleunder extreme circumstances.[47]
Human interaction
edit-
Humpback whale and kayakers offAvila Beach, California
-
Humpback whales offSaint-Gilles, Réunion
Whale watchingis carried out on every continent, with an estimated 13 million people participating in 2008.[48]This, when combined with the sustained increase in boat vessel traffic, has likely affected the surface activity of cetaceans. When boats and other whale watching vessels approach, most cetaceans will either avoid or seek interactions. The occasions where no effect is seen is predominantly when the cetaceans are travelling or feeding, but not when they are showing surface activity.[49]In the case of avoidance, the animals may dive rather than staying submerged near the surface or move horizontally away from the vessels.[50]For example, when sperm whales are approached by boats they surface less, shorten the intervals between breathes and do not show their fluke before diving as often.[49]Cetaceans may also reduce their acrobatic surfacing behaviours, such as whenhumpback whalegroups without calves are approached by vessels to within 300 m.[51]Avoidance behaviour is typical of whales, but interactions are more common in whale groups that contain calves[50]and also in the smallerodontocetes.For example, studies onkiller whalesin North America have shown that the focal animals increased their tail-slapping behaviour when approached by boats within 100 m, and that 70% of surface active behaviours (SABs) in these orca were seen when a boat was within 225 m.[52]Similarly,dusky dolphinsalso jump, change direction and form tighter groups more when boats are present, particularly when they do not adhere to the regulations about approach.[53]As an increase in SABs is beneficial to thewhale watchingtours' participants, the tours may be encouraged to approach cetaceans closer than recommended by guidelines. There is a lack of understanding about the long-term effects of whale-watching on the behaviour of cetaceans, but it is theorised that it may cause avoidance of popular sites,[51]or a decrease in the energy budget for individuals involved.[50]
See also
edit
References
edit- ^[1][permanent dead link ],FAO Marine Mammals of the World, Suborder Odontoceti.
- ^Whitehead Lab,Publications List,retrieved2015-05-25
- ^Ware, C.; Friedlaender, A. S.; Nowacek, D. P. (2011), "Shallow and deep lunge feeding of humpback whales in fjords of the West Antarctic Peninsula",Marine Mammal Science,27(3): 587–605,Bibcode:2011MMamS..27..587W,doi:10.1111/j.1748-7692.2010.00427.x
- ^Paul and Michael Albert (26 June 2005)."The Flying Mobulas of the Sea of Cortez".Archived fromthe originalon 4 May 2013.
- ^abcThe Dodo,This is how whales break through the surface of the water,archived fromthe originalon 2015-05-25,retrieved2015-05-25
- ^Breaching,Conscious Breath Adventures,retrieved2015-05-25
- ^Hal Whitehead (2003),Sperm whales: social evolution in the ocean,University of Chicago Press,p.179,ISBN978-0-226-89517-8
- ^"Whale Behaviour".Animal Corner. 26 February 2015.
- ^abWhitehead, Hal (2003),Sperm Whales: Social Evolution in the Ocean,Chicago: University of Chicago Press, p. 181,ISBN978-0-226-89517-8
- ^abcUSCB ScienceLine,Why do whales and other sea mammals breach?
- ^abcdefghAu, D.; Weihs, D. (1980), "At high speeds dolphins save energy by leaping.",Nature,284(5756): 548–550,Bibcode:1980Natur.284..548A,doi:10.1038/284548a0,S2CID4334691
- ^Hui, C. A. (1987), "The porpoising of penguins an energy-conserving behaviour for respiratory ventilation?",Canadian Journal of Zoology,65:209–211,doi:10.1139/z87-031
- ^abcWeihs, D. (2002), "Dynamics of dolphin porpoising revisited",Integrative and Comparative Biology,42(5): 1071–1078,doi:10.1093/icb/42.5.1071,PMID21680390
- ^Heithaus, M. R. (2001), "Shark attacks on bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus) in Shark Bay, Western Australia: attack rate, bite scar frequencies, and attack seasonality",Marine Mammal Science,17(3): 526–539,Bibcode:2001MMamS..17..526H,doi:10.1111/j.1748-7692.2001.tb01002.x
- ^Lusseau, D. (2003), "Effects of tour boats on the behavior of bottlenose dolphins: using Markov chains to model anthropogenic impacts",Conservation Biology,17(6): 1785–1793,Bibcode:2003ConBi..17.1785L,doi:10.1111/j.1523-1739.2003.00054.x,S2CID83589031
- ^abWeihs, D.; Fish, F. E.; Nicastro, A. J. (2007), "Mechanics of remora removal by dolphin spinning",Marine Mammal Science,23(3): 707–714,Bibcode:2007MMamS..23..707W,doi:10.1111/j.1748-7692.2007.00131.x
- ^[2],"Why do dolphins jump out of the water?", Dolphin World FAQ.
- ^Miller, L. J.; Solangi, M.; Kuczaj, S. A. (2010), "Seasonal and Diurnal Patterns of Behavior Exhibited by Atlantic Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in the Mississippi Sound ",Ethology,116(12): 1127–1137,Bibcode:2010Ethol.116.1127M,doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.2010.01824.x
- ^abcdeWilliams, T. M.; Friedl, W. A.; Fong, M. L.; Yamada, R. M.; Sedivy, P.; Huan, J. E. (1992), "Travel at low energetic cost by swimming and wave-riding bottlenose dolphins",Nature,355(6363): 821–823,Bibcode:1992Natur.355..821W,doi:10.1038/355821a0,PMID1538760,S2CID4281153
- ^Rafael Avitabile (10 September 2018)."Fishing charter crew captures orca pod playing in boat's wake".NBC News - San Diego.Retrieved5 November2020.
- ^abDahlheim, M. E. (1980), "Killer whales observed bowriding",The Murrelet,61(2): 78–80,JSTOR3535038
- ^abcRitter, F. (2003),"Interactions of cetaceans with whale watching boats–implications for the management of whale watching tourism"(PDF),MEER EV, Berlin, Germany,archived fromthe original(PDF)on 2015-05-24
- ^Hawkins, E.; Gartside, D. F. (2009), "Interactive behaviours of bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus) during encounters with vessels",Aquatic Mammals,35(2): 259–268,doi:10.1578/AM.35.2.2009.259
- ^"La baleine s'amuse by Gilles Macé".PBase.Retrieved24 September2018.
- ^Quất điền tin lành lợi (7 March 2014)."クジラ の đại đàn".Archivedfrom the original on 2021-12-21.Retrieved24 September2018– via YouTube.
- ^"Boná Island and Panama Canal, Panama".Expeditions.January 14, 2016.
- ^"Bowriding Humpback Whales – Log Transcript".PBS.December 30, 2002.
- ^"2 gray whales catch waves near Mission Beach".FOX5 San Diego.February 3, 2016.
- ^abGray Whale Spyhop,National Geographic, archived fromthe originalon 2015-05-25,retrieved2015-05-25
- ^Baldwin, Marc."Elasmobranch Behaviour".Wildlife Online.RetrievedAugust 7,2020.
- ^Keartes, Sarah (October 5, 2016)."What is the shark-steps-on-Lego shark actually doing?".Earth Touch News.RetrievedAugust 7,2020.
- ^Ng, S. L.; Leung, S. (2003), "Behavioral response of Indo-Pacific humpback dolphin (Sousa chinensis) to vessel traffic",Marine Environmental Research,56(5): 555–567,Bibcode:2003MarER..56..555N,doi:10.1016/S0141-1136(03)00041-2,PMID12927738
- ^abPitman, R. L.; Durban, J. W. (2012),"Cooperative hunting behavior, prey selectivity and prey handling by pack ice killer whales (Orcinus orca), type B, in Antarctic Peninsula waters",Marine Mammal Science,28(1): 16–36,Bibcode:2012MMamS..28...16P,doi:10.1111/j.1748-7692.2010.00453.x
- ^abcWeinrich, Mason T.; Schilling, Mark R.; Belt, Cynthia R. (December 1992), "Evidence for acquisition of a novel feeding behaviour: lobtail feeding in humpback whales,Megaptera novaeangliae",Animal Behaviour,44(6): 1059–1072,doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80318-5,S2CID53270820
- ^Dr Stavros Kaniklides,The effects of volcanic tsunamis on marine mammals,retrieved2015-05-26
- ^Kavanagh, A. S., Owen, K., Williamson, M. J., Noad, M. J., Goldizen, A. W., Kniest, E., Cato, D. H., Dunlop, R. A. (January 2017)."Evidence for the functions of surface-active behaviors in humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae)".Marine Mammal Science.33(1): 313–334.Bibcode:2017MMamS..33..313K.doi:10.1111/mms.12374– via ResearchGate.
{{cite journal}}
:CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^abDunlop, R. A.; Cato, D. H.; Noad, M. J. (2008), "Non-song acoustic communication in migrating humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae). ",Marine Mammal Science,24(3): 613–629,Bibcode:2008MMamS..24..613D,doi:10.1111/j.1748-7692.2008.00208.x
- ^abDeakos, M. H. (2012), "Humpback whale (Megaptera novaeangliae) communication: The context and potential functions of pec-slapping behavior on the Hawaiian wintering grounds. ",Doctoral Dissertation, University of Hawaii at Manoa(Thesis),hdl:10125/7066
- ^"North Atlantic right whale".Arkive. Archived fromthe originalon 2008-12-08.Retrieved2015-05-04.
- ^Harris, Tom (13 July 2001),"How Whales Work",Howstuffworks,retrieved2006-11-27
- ^"Whales"(PDF),Marine Discovery Centre, Henley Beach, S. A., archived fromthe original(PDF)on 28 August 2006,retrieved17 November2024
- ^Miller, P. J. O.; Aoki, K.; Rendell, L. E.; Amano, M. (2008), "Stereotypical resting behaviour of the sperm whale.",Current Biology,18(1): R21–R23,Bibcode:2008CBio...18..R21M,doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.11.003,PMID18177706,S2CID10587736
- ^Lyamin, O. I.; Mukhametov, L. M.; Seigel, J. M.; Nazarenko, E. A.; Polyakova, I. G.; Shpak, O. V. (2002), "Unihemispheric slow wave sleep and the state of the eyes in a white whales",Behavioural Brain Research,129(1–2): 125–129,doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00346-1,PMC8788623,PMID11809503,S2CID27009685
- ^"Why do the Humpback Whales visit Hervey Bay?",The Oceania Project,retrieved2015-05-13
- ^abWeilgart, L. S.; Whitehead, H. (1990), "Vocalizations of the North Atlantic pilot whale (Globicephala melas) as related to behavioral contexts ",Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology,26(6): 399–402,Bibcode:1990BEcoS..26..399W,doi:10.1007/BF00170896,S2CID34187605
- ^"The Deepest Divers",Oceanus Magazine, Woods Hole Oceanographic Institute.
- ^Quick, Nicola J.; Cioffi, William R.; Shearer, Jeanne M.; Fahlman, Andreas; Read, Andrew J. (15 September 2020)."Extreme diving in mammals: first estimates of behavioural aerobic dive limits in Cuvier's beaked whales".The Journal of Experimental Biology.223(18): jeb222109.doi:10.1242/jeb.222109.PMID32967976.S2CID221886321.
- ^O'Connor, S.; Campbell, R.; Cortez, H.; Knowles, T. (2009),"Whale Watching Worldwide: tourism numbers, expenditures and expanding economic benefits: a special report from the International Fund for Animal Welfare, Yarmouth MA, USA.",Economists at Large,archived fromthe originalon August 1, 2012
- ^abGordon, J.; Leaper, R.; Hartley, F. G.; Chappell, O. (1992),"Effects of whale-watching vessels on the surface and underwater acoustic behaviour of sperm whales off Kaikoura, New Zealand."(PDF),Wellington, N.Z.: Head Office, Department of Conservation,retrieved2015-05-20
- ^abcStamation, K. A.; Croft, D. B.; Shaughnessy, P. D.; Waples, K. A.; Briggs, S. V. (2010), "Behavioral responses of humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) to whale-watching vessels on the southeastern coast of Australia",Marine Mammal Science,26(1): 98–122,Bibcode:2010MMamS..26...98S,doi:10.1111/j.1748-7692.2009.00320.x
- ^abCorkeron, P. J. (1995), "Humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) in Hervey Bay, Queensland: behaviour and responses to whale-watching vessels",Canadian Journal of Zoology,73(7): 1290–1299,doi:10.1139/z95-153
- ^Noren, D. P.; Johnson, A. H.; Rehder, D.; Larson, A. (2009), "Close approaches by vessels elicit surface active behaviors by southern resident killer whales.",Endangered Species Research,8(3): 179–192,doi:10.3354/esr00205
- ^Barr, K.; Slooten, E. (1999),"Effects of tourism on dusky dolphins at Kaikoura."(PDF),Wellington: Department of Conservation,retrieved2015-05-20
Further reading
edit- Würsig, B.; Dorsey, E.M.; Richardson, W.J.; Wells, R.S. (1989),"Feeding, aerial and play behaviour of the bowhead whale,Balaena mysticetus,summering in the Beaufort Sea "(PDF),Aquatic Mammals,15(1): 27–37