Jump to content

Radial arm maze

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
A simple homemade eight-arm radial arm maze with sidewalls to prevent interarm traverses

Theradial arm mazewas designed by Olton and Samuelson in 1976 to measurespatial learningandmemoryin rats.[1]The original apparatus consists of eight equidistantly spaced arms, each about 4 feet long, and all radiating from a small circular central platform (later versions have used as few as three[2]and as many as 48 arms[3]). At the end of each arm there is a food site, the contents of which are not visible from the central platform.

Two types of memory that are assessed during the performance in this task are reference memory andworking memory.Reference memory is assessed when the rats only visit the arms of the maze which contains the reward. The failure to do so will result in reference memory error. Working memory is assessed when the rats enter each arm a single time. Re-entry into the arms would result in a working memory error.[4]

The design ensures that, after checking for food at the end of each arm, the rat is always forced to return to the central platform before making another choice. As a result, the rat always has eight possible options. Elaborate controls are used to ensure that the rats are not simply using theirsense of smell,either to sense unclaimed food objects or to sense their own tracks.

Olton and Samuelson found that rats have excellent memories for visited and unvisited arms; they made, on average, about 7.0 novel entries in their first 8 choices, and thus were 88% correct.[1]Chance performance with eight arms would be 5.3 novel entries in the first 8 choices (66% correct). Olton and Samuelson also found when they switched some already-visited arms into as yet unvisited locations partway through a trial, that the rats tended to visit as-yet unvisited locations even when doing so meant running down arms that had already been traversed, and tended to avoid arms that had not yet been traversed but were now in previously visited locations.[1]It therefore seems that in remembering locations on the radial arm maze, rats do not rely on local intra-maze cues, but rather on extra-maze cues.

Uses

[edit]

The maze has since been used extensively by researchers interested in studying the spatial learning andspatial memoryof animals. For example, Olton and colleagues found that performance declined only slightly to 82% novel entries in the first 17 entries on a 17-arm maze.[5]Roberts found no decline in the percentage of correct choices as the number of arms on a radial maze were increased from 8 to 16 and then to 24.[6]Cole and Chappell-Stephenson, using a radial maze with food locations ranging from 8 to 48, estimated the limit of spatial memory in rats to be between 24 and 32 locations.[3]

In one experiment utilizing the radial arm maze, it was shown that spatial relations among hidden target sites control spatial decisions that rats make and are unrelated to visual or perceptual cues that are related to certain locations.[7][clarification needed]

In another experiment, it was shown that subjects with Williams syndrome performed significantly worse compared to control subjects in multiple parameters such as visuo-spatial memory, general spatial function, and procedural competence.[8]

In mice, large differences in learning ability exist among differentinbred strains.[9]These differences appear to be correlated with the size of a part of the hippocampalmossy fiberprojection.[10]

The radial arm maze has shown to be practicable to investigate how drugs affect memory performance. It has also been shown to be useful in distinguishing the cognitive effects of an array of toxicants.[11]

The radial arm maze has also been use for several studies in children and adults.[12]A particular study led by L. Mandolesi used subjects with William's Syndrome (WS) because of the interest placed on their cognitive profile. There is a dissociation between spatial processing and visuo-object processing suggests that in WS subjects spatial functions are more severely impaired than visuo-perceptual ones. This is what RAM tests for.[12]

Limitations

[edit]

Various different types of mazes are used to assess memory. It is believed that performance of animals in one type of maze cannot be generalized to other mazes because all mazes require animals to utilize a different set of skills.[13]

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^abcOlton, D.S.; Samuelson, R.J. (1976). "Remembrance of places passed: Spatial memory in rats".Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes.2(2): 97–116.doi:10.1037/0097-7403.2.2.97.
  2. ^Lenck-Santini PP, Save E, Poucet B (2001). "Place-cell firing does not depend on the direction of turn in a Y-maze alternation task".Eur J Neurosci.13(5): 1055–8.doi:10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01481.x.PMID11264680.S2CID30940014.
  3. ^abCole, M.R.; Chappell-Stephenson, Robyn (2003)."Exploring the limits of spatial memory using very large mazes".Learning & Behavior.31(4): 349–368.doi:10.3758/BF03195996.PMID14733483.
  4. ^E. Tarragon, L. Lopez, F. Ros-Bernal, J.E. Yuste, V. Ortiz-Cullera, E. Martin, E. Schenker, F. Aujard, R.Bordet, J.C. Richardson, M.T. Herrero,"The Radial Arm Maze (RAM) for the Evaluation of Working and Reference Memory Deficits in the Diurnal Rodent Octodon degus."Proceedings of Measuring Behavior2012 (Utrecht, The Netherlands, August 28-31, 2012).
  5. ^Olton, D.S; Collison, C.; Werz, M.A. (1977). "Spatial memory and radial arm maze performance of rats".Learning & Motivation.8(3): 289–314.doi:10.1016/0023-9690(77)90054-6.
  6. ^Roberts, W.A. (1979). "Spatial memory in the rat on a hierarchical maze".Learning & Motivation.10(2): 117–140.doi:10.1016/0023-9690(79)90040-7.
  7. ^Brown, Michael F.; Gary W. Guimetti (2006)."Spatial pattern learning in the radial arm maze".Learning & Behavior.34(1): 102–108.doi:10.3758/BF03192875.PMID16786888.
  8. ^Mandolesi, L.; Addona, F.; Foti, F.; Menghini, F.; Petrosini, L.; Vicari, S. (2009). "Spatial competences in Williams syndrome: a radial arm maze study".Int. J. Devl Neuroscience.27(3): 205–213.doi:10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2009.01.004.PMID19429385.S2CID29696910.
  9. ^Crusio, Wim E.;Schwegler, Herbert (2013). "Chapter 29: Radial maze". In Crusio, Wim E.; Sluyter, Frans;Gerlai, Robert T.;Pietropaolo, Susanna (eds.).Behavioral Genetics of the Mouse.Cambridge Handbooks in Behavioral Genetics. Vol. 1. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 299–303.ISBN978-1-107-03481-5.
  10. ^W. E. Crusio& H. Schwegler (2005)."Learning spatial orientation tasks in the radial-maze and structural variation in the hippocampus in inbred mice".Behavioral and Brain Functions.1(1): 3.doi:10.1186/1744-9081-1-3.PMC1143776.PMID15916698.
  11. ^Levin, ED (1988). "Psychopharmacological effects in the radial-arm maze".Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews.12(2): 169–75.doi:10.1016/S0149-7634(88)80008-3.PMID2902540.S2CID31161513.
  12. ^abL. Mandolesi; F. Addona; F. Foti; D. Menghini; L. Petrosini; S. Vicari, "Spatial competences in Williams syndrome: a radial arm maze study",International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience,May 2009, 27 (3), 205-213
  13. ^Hodges, H. (1995). "Maze Procedures: the radial-arm and water maze compared".Cognitive Brain Research.3(3–4): 167–181.doi:10.1016/0926-6410(96)00004-3.PMID8806020.