Jump to content

Conscientiousness

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Conscientiousnessis thepersonality traitof being responsible,carefulordiligent.Conscientiousness implies a desire to do a task well, and to take obligations to others seriously. Conscientious people tend to beefficientand organized as opposed toeasy-goinganddisorderly.They tend to showself-discipline,actdutifully,andaim for achievement;they displayplannedrather than spontaneous behavior; and they are generally dependable. Conscientiousness manifests in characteristic behaviors such as being neat,systematic,careful,thorough,anddeliberate(tending to think carefully before acting).[1]

Conscientiousness is one of the five traits of both theFive Factor Modeland theHEXACO modelof personality and is an aspect of what has traditionally been referred to as havingcharacter.Conscientious individuals are generallyhard-workingandreliable.When taken to an extreme, they may also be "workaholics",perfectionists,andcompulsivein their behavior.[2]People who score low on conscientiousness tend to be laid back, less goal-oriented, and less driven by success, if they also score high on Big Five Agreeableness; otherwise, they are also more likely to engage inanti-socialbehavior and commitblue-collared crimesandcrimes of passion.[3]

Personality models

[edit]

Conscientiousness is one of the five majordimensionsin theBig Five modelofpersonality(also calledFive Factor Modelor OCEAN), which consists ofopenness to experience,conscientiousness,extraversion,agreeableness,andneuroticism.Two of manypersonality teststhat assess these traits are Costa and McCrae'sNEO PI-R[4]andGoldberg's NEO-IPIP.[5]According to these models, conscientiousness is acontinuousdimension of personality, rather than acategorical"type"of person.

In the NEO framework, Conscientiousness has six facets:Competence,Order,Dutifulness,Achievement Striving,Self-Discipline,andDeliberation.Other models suggest a smaller set of two "aspects":orderliness andindustriousnessforman intermediate level of organization[clarification needed],with orderliness associated with the desire to keep things organized and tidy and industriousness being more associated withproductivityandwork ethic.[6]

Other Big Five personality traits such as lowextraversion,highagreeableness,lowopenness,and lowneuroticismare linked to high conscientiousness.[4]

Conscientiousness also appears in other models of personality, such asCloninger'sTemperament and Character Inventory,in which it is related to bothself-directednessandpersistence.[7]The traits ofrule consciousnessandperfectionismare included inCattell's16 PF model.It[ambiguous]is negatively associated withimpulsivesensation-seekingin Zuckerman'salternative fivemodel.

Traitsassociated with conscientiousness are frequently assessed by self-report integrity tests given by various corporations to prospective employees.

Origin

[edit]

Terms such as "hard-working", "reliable", and "persevering" describe desirable aspects of character. Because conscientiousness was once believed to be a moral evaluation, it was overlooked as a psychological attribute. The reality of individual differences in conscientiousness has now been clearly established by studies of cross-observer agreement. Peer and expert ratings confirm the self-reports that people make about their degrees of conscientiousness. Furthermore, both self-reports and observer ratings of conscientiousness predict real-life outcomes such as academic success.[8]

During most of the 20th century, psychologists believed that personality traits could be divided into two categories:temperamentandcharacter.Temperament traits were thought to be biologically based, whereas character traits were thought to be learned either during childhood or throughout life. With the advent of theFFM(Five-Factor Model), behavior geneticists began systematic studies of the full range of personality traits, and it soon became clear that all five factors are substantially heritable. Identical twins showed very similar personality traits even when they had been separated at birth and raised apart, and this was true for both character traits and temperament traits. Parents and communities influence the ways in which conscientiousness is expressed, but they apparently do not influence its level.[9]

Measurement

[edit]

A person's level of conscientiousness is generally assessed using self-report measures, although peer-reports and third-party observation can also be used. Self-report measures are eitherlexical[1]or based onstatements.[10]Deciding which measure of either type to use in research is determined by an assessment of psychometric properties and the time and space constraints of the study being undertaken.

Lexical

[edit]

Lexical measuresuse adjectives that reflect conscientiousness traits, such as "efficient" and "systematic", and are very space- and time-efficient for research purposes. Goldberg[11]developed a 20-word measure as part of his 100-word Big Five markers. Saucier[12]developed a briefer 8-word measure as part of his 40-word mini-markers. Thompson[1]revised these measures to develop the International English Mini-Markers which has superior validity and reliability in populations both within and outside North America. Internal consistency reliability of the International English Mini-Markers for the Conscientiousness measure for native English-speakers is reported as.90, that for non-native English-speakers is.86.[1]

Statement

[edit]

Statement measurestend to comprise more words than lexical measures, so consume more research instrument space and more respondent time to complete. Respondents are asked the extent to which they, for example, often forget to put things back in their proper place, or are careful to avoid making mistakes.[10]Some statement-based measures of conscientiousness have similarly acceptable psychometric properties in North American populations to lexical measures, but their generallyemicdevelopment makes them less suited to use in other populations.[13]For instance, statements in colloquial North American English likeOften forget to put things back in their proper placeorAm careful to avoid making mistakescan be hard for non-native English-speakers to understand, suggesting internationally validated measures might be more appropriate for research conducted with non-North Americans.

Behavior

[edit]

Development

[edit]

Currently,[when?]little is known about conscientiousness in young children because the self-report inventories typically used to assess it are not appropriate for that age group. There are likely individual differences in this factor at an early age. It is known, for example, that some children haveattention deficit/hyperactivity disorder,which is characterized in part by problems with concentration, organization, and persistence.[14]Longitudinal and cross-sectional studies suggest that conscientiousness is relatively low among adolescents but increases between 18 and 30 years of age. Conscientiousness generally increases with age from 21 to 60, though the rate of increase is slow.

Individual differences are strongly preserved, meaning that a careful, neat, and scrupulous 30-year-old is likely to become a careful, neat, and scrupulous 80-year-old.[9]

Daily life

[edit]

People who score high on the trait of conscientiousness tend to be more organized and less cluttered in their homes and offices. For example, their books tend to be neatly shelved in Alpha betical order, or categorized by topic, rather than scattered around the room. Their clothes tend to be folded and arranged in drawers or closets instead of lying on the floor. The presence of planners and to-do lists are also signs of conscientiousness. Their homes tend to have better lighting than the homes of people who score low on this trait.[15]

Academic and workplace performance

[edit]

Conscientiousness correlates with successful academic performance in students and workplace performance among managers and workers.[16]Low levels of conscientiousness are strongly associated withprocrastination.[17]Conscientiousness has a moderate to large positive correlation with performance in the workplace,[18]and in contrast, aftergeneral mental abilityis taken into account, the other fourBig Five personality traitsdo not aid in predicting career success.[19]: 169 [verification needed]

Conscientious employees are generally more reliable, more motivated, and harder working. They have lower rates of absenteeism andcounterproductive work behaviorssuch as stealing and fighting with other employees.[20]Furthermore, conscientiousness is the personality trait that mostly[ambiguous]correlates with performance across all categories of jobs.[21] Agreeablenessand emotional stability may also be important, particularly in jobs that involve a significant amount of social interaction.[22] Of all manager/leader types, top executives show the lowest level of rule-following, a conscientious trait.[23]Conscientiousness is not always positively related tojob performance;sometimes the opposite is true. Being too conscientious could lead to taking too much time in making urgent decisions and to being too attached to the rules and lacking innovation.[24]

Subjective well-being

[edit]

A 2008 meta-analysis found that conscientiousness has a positive relationship with subjective well-being, particularly satisfaction with life. Highly conscientious people tend to be happier with their lives than those who score low on this trait.[25] Although conscientiousness is generally seen as a positive trait to possess, research suggests that in some situations it may be harmful for well-being. In a prospective study of 9,570 people over four years, highly conscientious people suffered[how?]more than twice as much if they became unemployed.[26]The authors suggested this may be due to conscientious people making different attributions about why they became unemployed, or through experiencing stronger reactions following failure. This finding is consistent with perspectives which see no trait as inherently positive or negative, but rather the consequences of the trait being dependent on the situation and concomitant goals and motivations.[27]

Problematic life outcomes

[edit]

Low conscientiousness is linked to anti-social behavior, blue-collared crimes, and crimes of passion,[3]as well as unemployment and homelessness.[20]Low conscientiousness and low agreeableness taken together are also associated withsubstance use disorders.[28]People low in conscientiousness have difficulty saving money and their risky borrowing practices make them fall prey to subprime andpredatory lendingmore often than conscientious people. High conscientiousness is associated with more careful planning of shopping trips and less impulse buying of unneeded items.[20]While conscientiousness is generally linked to a lower level of anti-social behaviour, it is not always the case, and conscientiousness is not always negatively correlated with criminality. It has been shown that conscientiousness is positively correlated with business,white-collared,andpremeditatedcriminal behavior.[29]

Health and longevity

[edit]

According to a multi-decade study begun in 1921 by psychologistLewis Termanon over 1,500 gifted adolescent Californians, "The strongest predictor of long life was conscientiousness."[30]Specific behaviors associated with low conscientiousness may explain its influence onlongevity.Nine different behaviors that are among the leading causes of mortality—alcohol use, disordered eating (including obesity), drug use, lack of exercise,risky sexual behavior,risky driving, tobacco use, violence, and suicide—are all predicted by low conscientiousness. Health behaviors are more strongly correlated with the "conventionality" rather than the "impulse-control" aspect of conscientiousness. Apparently, social norms influence many health-relevant behaviors, such as healthy diet, exercise, not smoking, and moderate drinking, and highly conscientious people adhere the most strongly to these norms.[20]Conscientiousness is positively related to health behaviors[31]such as regular visits to a doctor, checking smoke alarms, and adherence to medication regimens. Such behavior may better safeguard health and prevent disease.[20][better source needed]The only known health benefits of low conscientiousness are relaxation and reduced anxiety.

Relationships

[edit]

Relationship quality is positively associated with partners' levels of conscientiousness. Highly conscientious people are less likely to get divorced due to their own fault. Conscientiousness is associated with lower rates of behavior associated with at-fault divorce, such as extramarital affairs, spousal abuse, and alcohol use disorder. Conscientious behaviors may have a direct influence on relationship quality, as people low in conscientiousness are less responsible, less responsive to their partners, more condescending, and less likely to hold back offensive comments. More conscientious people are better at managing conflict and tend to provoke fewer disagreements, perhaps because they elicit less criticism due to their well-controlled and responsible behavior.[20][better source needed]

Intelligence

[edit]

Conscientiousness has distinct sub-components, including two major branches (i.e., orderliness and industriousness). The largest set of meta-analyses found that these branches of conscientiousness have disparate connections with cognitive abilities. Orderliness has negative correlations with some cognitive abilities (e.g., fluid reasoning abilities, math knowledge, lexical knowledge, and science knowledge) whereas industriousness has strikingly positive correlations with multiple abilities (e.g., general mental ability, memory, processing speed, and knowledge).[32]

Some previous studies in selected samples (e.g., job candidates,[33]university students) have found negative correlations between conscientiousness and intelligence. One potential explanation is that these negative correlations may be artificiallycreated by the selectionof the sample. For example, with students who are low on one trait and high on the other (negative relationship) using the strong one to compensate and pass the selection, while students who are low on both (positive relationship) are removed by the selection.[34]

Political attitudes

[edit]

Conscientiousness correlates with conservative political attitudes.[35]

Because conscientiousness is positively related to job performance,[36]conservative service workers have been shown to earn higher ratings, evaluations, and tips than their liberal counterparts.[37]

Although right-wing authoritarianism is one of the most powerful predictors of prejudice, a large scale meta-analysis found that conscientiousness itself is uncorrelated with general prejudice.[38]Rebellion against control is significantly negatively correlated with conscientiousness.[39]

Hence, conscientiousness is associated only with rule compliance, obedience, and integrity, not necessarily with right-wing authoritarianism, which requires reduced Big Five Agreeableness as well.[40]

Creativity

[edit]

The orderliness/dependability subfactors (order, dutifulness, and deliberation) of conscientiousness correlate negatively with creativity while the industriousness/achievement subfactors (competence, achievement striving, and self-discipline) correlate positively.[41]People who score high on the order subfactor of conscientiousness show less innovative behavior, especially if coupled with a low score on the industrious subfactor.[42]Group conscientiousness[definition needed]has a negative effect on group performance during creative tasks.[43]Groups with only conscientious members have difficulty solving open-ended problems.[44]

Adaptability

[edit]

A study from 2006 found that those scoring low on conscientiousness make better decisions after unanticipated changes in the context of a task. Specifically, the subfactors order, dutifulness, and deliberation negatively correlated with decision-making quality, but not[ambiguous]competence, achievement striving, and self-discipline.[45]

Religiosity

[edit]

General religiosity was mainly correlated with high Agreeableness and Conscientiousness among the Big Five traits.[46]

Conscience

[edit]

Conscientiousness in psychology is defined, as noted above, by notions such as "orderliness and industriousness", not by reference to the ordinary language notion ofconscience,or such as is referred to in the legal doctrine ofequity,by concern with preventing unconscionable actions.

Geography

[edit]

United States

[edit]

Average levels of conscientiousness vary bystate in the United States.People living in the central part, including the states ofKansas,Nebraska,Oklahoma,andMissouri,on average have higher scores than people living in other regions. People in the southwestern states ofNew Mexico,Utah,andArizonaalso have relatively high average scores on conscientiousness. Among the eastern states,Floridais the only one that scores in the top ten for this personality trait. The four states with the lowest scores on conscientiousness on average were, in descending order,Rhode Island,Hawaii,Maine,andAlaska.[47]

Great Britain

[edit]

A large scale survey of residents of Great Britain found that average levels of all the Big Five, including conscientiousness, vary across regional districts in England, Wales, and Scotland. High levels of conscientiousness were found throughout much of Southern England, scattered areas of the Midlands, and most of the Scottish Highlands. Low levels of conscientiousness were observed in London, Wales, and parts of the North of England. Higher mean levels of regional conscientiousness were positively correlated with voting for Toryism and the Conservative Party, and negatively correlated with voting for the Labour Party, in the 2005 and 2010 elections, and also correlated with a higher proportion of married residents, with higher life expectancy for men and women, fewer long-term health problems, and with lower rates of mortality from stroke, cancer, and heart disease. Higher regional conscientiousness was also correlated with lower median annual income in 2011.[48]

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^abcdThompson, E.R. (October 2008). "Development and Validation of an International English Big-Five Mini-Markers".Personality and Individual Differences.45(6): 542–548.doi:10.1016/j.paid.2008.06.013.
  2. ^Carter, Nathan L.; Guan, Li; Maples, Jessica L.; Williamson, Rachel L.; Miller, Joshua D. (2015). "The downsides of extreme conscientiousness for psychological wellbeing: The role of obsessive compulsive tendencies".Journal of Personality.84(4): 510–522.doi:10.1111/jopy.12177.PMID25858019.
  3. ^abOzer, D. J.; Benet-Martínez, V. (2006). "Personality and the prediction of consequential outcomes".Annual Review of Psychology.57:401–421.doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190127.PMID16318601.S2CID20451934.
  4. ^abCosta, P.T.; McCrae, R.R. (1992).NEO personality Inventory professional manual.Odessa, Fla.: Psychological Assessment Resources.
  5. ^"International Personality Item Pool".ipip.ori.org.
  6. ^DeYoung, C.G.; Quilty, L. C.; Peterson, J.B. (2007). "Between facets and domains: 10 aspects of the Big Five".Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.93(5): 880–896.doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.880.PMID17983306.S2CID8261816.
  7. ^De Fruyt, F.; Van De Wiele, L.; Van Heeringen, C. (2000). "Cloninger's Psychobiological Model of Temperament and Character and the Five-Factor Model of Personality".Personality and Individual Differences.29(3): 441–452.doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00204-4.
  8. ^Kim, Lisa E.; Poropat, Arthur E.; MacCann, Carolyn (2016),"Conscientiousness in Education: Its Conceptualization, Assessment, and Utility",The Springer Series on Human Exceptionality,Cham: Springer International Publishing, pp. 155–185,doi:10.1007/978-3-319-28606-8_7,ISBN978-3-319-28604-4,retrieved2024-01-26
  9. ^abMcCrae, Robert (2004). "Conscientiousness".Encyclopedia of Applied Psychology.Academic Press. p. 470.[volume & issue needed]
  10. ^abGoldberg, L.R.; Johnson, J.A.; Eber, H.W.; et al. (2006). "The international personality item pool and the future of public-domain personality measures".Journal of Research in Personality.40(1): 84–96.doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2005.08.007.S2CID13274640.
  11. ^Goldberg, L.R. (1992). "The development of markers for the Big-Five factor structure".Psychological Assessment.4(1): 26–42.doi:10.1037/1040-3590.4.1.26.S2CID144709415.
  12. ^Saucier, G (1994). "Mini-Markers – a brief version of Goldberg's unipolar big-five markers".Journal of Personality Assessment.63(3): 506–516.doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa6303_8.PMID7844738.
  13. ^Piedmont, R.L.; Chae, J.H. (1997). "Cross-cultural generalizability of the five-factor model of personality – Development and validation of the NEO PI-R for Koreans".Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology.28(2): 131–155.doi:10.1177/0022022197282001.S2CID145053137.
  14. ^"Neurodevelopmental Disorders".NeuronUP.Retrieved2021-03-09.
  15. ^Gosling, S. (2008).Snoop: What your stuff says about you?.New York: Basic Books.
  16. ^Higgins, D.M.; Peterson, J.B.; Lee, A.; Pihl, R.O. (2007). "Prefrontal cognitive ability, intelligence, Big Five personality and the prediction of advanced academic and workplace performance".Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.93(2): 298–319.doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.2.298.PMID17645401.
  17. ^Dewitt, S.; Schouwenburg, H. C. (2002). "Procrastination, temptations, and incentives: The struggle between the present and the future in procrastinators and the punctual".European Journal of Personality.16(6): 469–489.doi:10.1002/per.461.S2CID7065818.
  18. ^
  19. ^Schmidt, Frank L.; Hunter, John (2004)."General Mental Ability in the World of Work: Occupational Attainment and Job Performance"(PDF).Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.86(1): 162–173.CiteSeerX10.1.1.394.8878.doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.162.PMID14717634.Archived fromthe original(PDF)on 2011-10-28.Retrieved2010-07-12.
  20. ^abcdefRoberts, B.W.; Jackson, J.J.; Fayard, J.V.; Edmonds, G.; Meints, J. (2009). "25. Conscientiousness". In Leary, Mark R.; Hoyle, Rick H. (eds.).Handbook of Individual Differences in Social Behavior.New York/London: The Guildford Press. pp. 257–273.ISBN978-1-59385-647-2.
  21. ^"Which Personality Traits Are Most Important to Employers?".Association for Psychological Science - APS.2014-09-25.Retrieved2021-03-16.
  22. ^Mount, M.K.; Barrick, M.R.; Stewart, G.L. (1998). "Five-factor model of personality and Performance in jobs involving interpersonal interactions".Human Performance.11(2): 145–165.doi:10.1080/08959285.1998.9668029.
  23. ^MacNab, Donald."New research shows leaders are persuasive rule-breakers"(PDF).Psychometrics Canada.[dead link]
  24. ^Tett, Robert P. (1 July 1998)."Is ConscientiousnessAlwaysPositively Related to Job Performance? ".Wright State University.
  25. ^Steel, Piers; Schmidt, Joseph; Shultz, Jonas (2008)."Refining the relationship between personality and Subjective well-being"(PDF).Psychological Bulletin.134(1): 138–161.doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.138.hdl:1880/47915.PMID18193998.Archived fromthe original(PDF)on 2019-02-14.Retrieved2012-07-11.
  26. ^Boyce, Christopher J.; Wood, Alex M.; Brown, Gordon D.A. (2010)."The dark side of conscientiousness: Conscientious people experience greater drops in life satisfaction following unemployment"(PDF).Journal of Research in Personality.44(4). Elsevier BV: 535–539.doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2010.05.001.ISSN0092-6566.Archived fromthe original(PDF)on 2011-02-24.
  27. ^Wood, Alex M.; Tarrier, Nicholas (2010)."Positive Clinical Psychology: A new vision and strategy for integrated research and practice"(PDF).Clinical Psychology Review.30(7). Elsevier BV: 819–829.doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.06.003.ISSN0272-7358.PMID20655136.S2CID11952756.Archived fromthe original(PDF)on 2011-07-17.
  28. ^Walton, K.E.; Roberts, B.W. (2004). "On the relationship between substance use and personality traits: abstainers are not maladjusted".J. Res. Personal.38(6): 515–35.doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2004.01.002.
  29. ^Blickle, G.; Schlegel, A. (2006). "Some Personality Correlates of Business White-Collar Crime".Applied Psychology.55(2): 220–233.doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597.2006.00226.x.
  30. ^
  31. ^Kern, Margaret L.; Friedman, Howard S. (2008–2009). "Do conscientious individuals live longer? A quantitative review".Health Psychology.27(5): 505–512.doi:10.1037/0278-6133.27.5.505.ISSN1930-7810.PMID18823176.
  32. ^Stanek, Kevin C.; Ones, Deniz S. (2023-06-06)."Meta-analytic relations between personality and cognitive ability".Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.120(23): e2212794120.Bibcode:2023PNAS..12012794S.doi:10.1073/pnas.2212794120.ISSN0027-8424.PMC10266031.PMID37252971.
  33. ^Moutafi, Joanna; Furnham, Adrian; Crump, John (January 2003)."Demographic and personality predictors of intelligence: a study using the Neo Personality Inventory and the Myers–Briggs Type Indicator".European Journal of Personality.17(1): 79–94.doi:10.1002/per.471.ISSN0890-2070.S2CID143599504.
  34. ^Murray, A.L.; Johnson, W.; McGue, M.; Iacono, W.G. (2014). "How are conscientiousness and cognitive ability related to one another? A re-examination of the intelligence compensation hypothesis".Personality and Individual Differences.70:17–22.doi:10.1016/j.paid.2014.06.014.
  35. ^
  36. ^
    • Brown, Tom J.; Mowen, John C.; Donavan, D. Todd; Licata, Jane W. (2002). "The Customer Orientation of Service Workers: Personality Trait Effects on Self-and Supervisor Performance Ratings".Journal of Marketing Research.39(1). SAGE Publications: 110–119.doi:10.1509/jmkr.39.1.110.18928.ISSN0022-2437.S2CID144095642.
    • Neal, Andrew; Yeo, Gillian; Koy, Annette; Xiao, Tania (2011-01-26). "Predicting the form and direction of work role performance from the Big 5 model of personality traits".Journal of Organizational Behavior.33(2). Wiley: 175–192.doi:10.1002/job.742.ISSN0894-3796.
  37. ^Davidson, Alexander; Theriault, Derek A. (2021-03-25). "How Consumer Experience Is Shaped by the Political Orientation of Service Providers".Journal of Consumer Psychology.31(4). Wiley: 792–800.doi:10.1002/jcpy.1233.ISSN1057-7408.S2CID233791649.
  38. ^
  39. ^Watson, David (2001)."Procrastination and the five-factor model: a facet level analysis".Personality and Individual Differences.30:149–158.doi:10.1016/s0191-8869(00)00019-2.
  40. ^Hogan, Robert; Johnson, John; Briggs, Stephen (1997).Handbook of Personality Psychology.Academic Press. p. 856.ISBN978-0-12-134645-4.
  41. ^Reiter-Palmon, Roni; Illies, Jody; Kobe-Cross, Lisa (2009)."Conscientiousness Is Not Always a Good Predictor of Performance: The Case of Creativity".The International Journal of Creativity & Problem Solving.19:27–45.
  42. ^Bakx, Nina."Which personality traits do innovative people possess?".Universiteit van Amsterdam. Archived fromthe originalon 2016-01-31.Retrieved2015-04-12.
  43. ^Robert, Christopher; Cheung, Yu Ha (April 2010). "An examination of the relationship between conscientiousness and group performance on a creative task".Journal of Research in Personality.44(2): 222–231.doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2010.01.005.
  44. ^Colarelli, Stephen (2003).No best way: an evolutionary perspective on human resource management.Westport, Conn.: Praeger. p. 159.ISBN978-0-275-95739-1.
  45. ^Lepine, Jeffrey; Colquitt, Jason; Erez, Amir (2000). "Adaptability to changing task context: effects of general cognitive ability, conscientiousness, and openness to experience".Personnel Psychology.53(3): 563–593.doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.2000.tb00214.x.
  46. ^Saroglou, Vassilis (2002). "Religion and the five-factors of personality: A meta-analytic review".Personality and Individual Differences.32:15–25.doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00233-6.
  47. ^
  48. ^Rentfrow, Peter J.; Jokela, Markus; Lamb, Michael E. (March 24, 2015)."Regional Personality Differences in Great Britain".PLOS ONE.10(3): e0122245.Bibcode:2015PLoSO..1022245R.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0122245.PMC4372610.PMID25803819.S2CID14827579.