Jump to content

False memory

Listen to this article
From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Inpsychology,afalse memoryis a phenomenon where someonerecallssomething that did not actually happen or recalls it differently from the way it actually happened.Suggestibility,activation of associated information, the incorporation of misinformation, andsource misattributionhave been suggested to be several mechanisms underlying a variety of types of false memory.

Early work[edit]

The false memory phenomenon was initially investigated by psychological pioneersPierre JanetandSigmund Freud.[1]

Freud was fascinated with memory and all the ways it could be understood, used, and manipulated. Some claim that his studies have been quite influential in contemporary memory research, including the research into the field of false memory.[2]Pierre Janetwas a Frenchneurologistalso credited with great contributions into memory research. Janet contributed to false memory through his ideas on dissociation and memory retrieval through hypnosis.[3]

In 1974,Elizabeth Loftusand John Palmer conducted a study[4]to investigate the effects of language on the development of false memory. The experiment involved two separate studies.

In the first study, 45 participants were randomly assigned to watch different videos of a car accident, in which separate videos had shown collisions at 20 mph (32 km/h), 30 mph (48 km/h) and 40 mph (64 km/h). Afterwards, participants filled out a survey. The survey asked the question, "About how fast were the cars going when they smashed into each other?" The question always asked the same thing, except the verb used to describe the collision varied. Rather thansmashed,other verbs used includedbumped,collided,hit,orcontacted.Participants estimated collisions of all speeds to average between 35 mph (56 km/h) to just below 40 mph (64 km/h). If actual speed was the main factor in estimate, it could be assumed that participants would have lower estimates for lower speed collisions. Instead, the word being used to describe the collision seemed to better predict the estimate in speed rather than the speed itself.[4]

The second experiment also showed participants videos of a car accident, but the phrasing of the follow-up questionnaire was critical in participant responses. 150 participants were randomly assigned to three conditions. Those in the first condition were asked the same question as the first study using the verbsmashed.The second group was asked the same question as the first study, replacingsmashedwithhit.The final group was not asked about the speed of the crashed cars. The researchers then asked the participants if they had seen any broken glass, knowing that there was no broken glass in the video. The responses to this question had shown that the difference between whether broken glass was recalled or not heavily depended on the verb used. A larger sum of participants in the "smashed" group declared that there was broken glass.

In this study, the first point brought up in discussion is that the words used to phrase a question can heavily influence the response given.[4]Second, the study indicates that the phrasing of a question can give expectations to previously ignored details, and therefore, a misconstruction of ourmemory recall.This indication supports false memory as an existing phenomenon.

Replications in different contexts (such as hockey games instead of car crashes) have shown that different scenarios require different framing effects to produce differing memories.[5]

Manifestations and types[edit]

Presuppositions and the misinformation effect[edit]

Apresuppositionis an implication through chosen language. If a person is asked, "What shade of blue was the wallet?", the questioner is, in translation, saying, "The wallet was blue. What shade was it?" The question's phrasing provides the respondent with a supposed "fact". This presupposition creates one of two separate effects: true effect and false effect.

  • Intrue effect,the implication was accurate: the wallet really was blue. That makes the respondent's recall stronger, more readily available, and easier to extrapolate from. A respondent is more likely to remember a wallet as blue if the prompt said that it was blue than if the prompt did not say so.
  • Infalse effect,the implication was actually false: the wallet was not blue even though the question asked what shade of blue it was. This convinces the respondent of its truth (i.e., that the walletwasblue), which affects their memory. It can also alter responses to later questions to keep them consistent with the false implication.

Regardless of the effect being true or false, the respondent is attempting to conform to the supplied information, because they assume it to be true.[6]

Loftus's meta-analysis on language manipulation studies suggested themisinformation effecttaking hold on the recall process and products of the human memory. Even the smallest adjustment in a question, such as the article preceding the supposed memory, could alter the responses. For example, having asked someone if they had seen "the" stop sign, rather than "a" stop sign, provided the respondent with a presupposition that there was a stop sign in the scene. This presupposition increased the number of people responding that they had indeed seen the stop sign.[7]

The strength of verbs used in conversation or questioning also has a similar effect on the memory; for example – the words "met", "bumped", "collided", "crashed", or "smashed" would all cause people to remember a car accident at different levels of intensity. The words "bumped", "hit", "grabbed", "smacked", or "groped" would all paint a different picture of a person in the memory of an observer of sexual harassment if questioned about it later. The stronger the word, the more intense the recreation of the experience in the memory is. This in turn could trigger further false memories to better fit the memory created (change how a person looks or how fast a vehicle was moving before a collision).[8]

Word lists[edit]

One can trigger false memories by presenting subjects a continuous list of words. When subjects were presented with a second version of the list and asked if the words had appeared on the previous list, they found that the subjects did not recognize the list correctly. When the words on the two lists were semantically related to each other (e.g. sleep/bed), it was more likely that the subjects did not remember the first list correctly and created false memories (Anisfeld & Knapp, 1963).[9]

In 1998, Kathleen McDermott and Henry Roediger III conducted a similar experiment. Their goal was to intentionally trigger false memories through word lists. They presented subjects with lists to study, all containing a large number of words that were semantically related to another word that was not found on the list. For example, if the word that they were trying to trigger was "river", the list would contain words such as flow, current, water, stream, bend, etc. They would then take the lists away and ask the subjects to recall the words on the lists. Almost every time, the false memory was triggered, and the subjects would end up recalling the target word as part of the list when it was never there. McDermott and Roediger even went as far as informing the subjects of the purpose and details of the experiment, and still the subjects would recall the non listed target word as part of the word list they had studied.[10]

Staged naturalistic events[edit]

Subjects were invited into an office and were told to wait there. After this, they had to recall the inventory of the visited office. Subjects recognized objects consistent with the "office schema", although they did not appear in the office. (Brewer & Treyens, 1981)[9]

In another study, subjects were presented with a situation where they witnessed a staged robbery. Half of the subjects witnessed the robbery live, while the other half watched a video of the robbery as it took place. After the event, they were sat down and asked to recall what had happened during the robbery. The results surprisingly showed that those who watched the video of the robbery actually recalled more information more accurately than those who were live on the scene. Still, false memory presented itself in ways such as subjects seeing things that would fit in a crime scene that were not there, or not recalling things that did not fit the crime scene. This happened with both parties, displaying the idea of staged naturalistic events.[11]

Relational processing[edit]

Memory retrieval has been associated with the brain's relational processing. In associating two events (in reference to false memory, say tying a testimony to a prior event), there are verbatim and gist representations. Verbatim matches to the individual occurrences (e.g., I do not like dogs because when I was five achihuahuabit me) and gist matches to general inferences (e.g., I do not like dogs because they are mean). Keeping in line with thefuzzy-trace theory,which suggests false memories are stored in gist representations (which retrieves both true and false recall), Storbeck & Clore (2005) wanted to see how change in mood affected the retrieval of false memories. After using the measure of a word association tool called theDeese–Roediger–McDermott paradigm(DRM), the subjects' moods were manipulated. Moods were either oriented towards being more positive, more negative, or were left untouched. Findings suggested that a more negative mood made critical details, stored in gist representation, more accessible.[12]

Mandela effect[edit]

TheBolognastation clock in Italy, subject of a collective false memory

Specific false memories can sometimes be shared by a large group of people. This phenomenon was dubbed the "Mandela effect" byparanormalresearcher Fiona Broome, who reported having vivid and detailed memories of news coverage ofSouth Africananti-apartheid leaderNelson Mandeladying in prison in the 1980s, despite Mandela actuallydying in 2013,decades after his release and after serving as President of South Africa from 1994 to 1999. Broome reported that hundreds of other people had written about having the same memory of Mandela's death,[13]some while he was still alive, and she speculated that the phenomenon could be evidence ofparallel realities.[14]

One well-documented example of shared false memories comes from a 2010 study that examined people familiar with the clock atBologna Centrale railway station,which was damaged in theBologna massacrebombing in August 1980. In the study, 92% of respondents falsely remembered the clock had remained stopped since the bombing, when, in fact, the clock was repaired shortly after the attack. Years later, the clock was again stopped and set to the time of the explosion, in observance and commemoration of the bombing.[15]

Other examples include memories of the respective title component of theBerenstain Bearschildren's books being spelled "Berenstein",[16][17]the logo of clothing brandFruit of the Loomfeaturing acornucopia,[18]Darth VadertellingLuke Skywalker,"Luke, I am your father" in the climax ofThe Empire Strikes Back(he actually says, "No,Iam your father "in response to Skywalker's assertion that Vader killed his father),[19]Mr. Monopolywearing amonocle,[20][21][22]and the existence of a 1990s movie titledShazaamstarring comedianSinbadas a genie[23](whichSnopessuggests could be aconfabulationof real memories, possibly including Sinbad wearing a genie-like costume during a TV marathon ofSinbad the Sailormovies in 1994,[24][25]the 1996 filmKazaamfeaturing a genie played by basketball starShaquille O'Neal,and the 1960s animated genie-themed seriesShazzan).[23][26][27]Likewise, false memories of Mandela's death could be explained as the subject conflating him withSteve Biko,another prominent South African anti-apartheid activist, who died in prison in 1977.[28][29][30]

Memesabout the Mandela effect and associated online jokes about atime traveleraltering the past and turning the current era into "a glitch" became popular in the United States in 2016.[31]

Scientists suggest that these are examples of false memories shaped by similar cognitive factors affecting multiple people and families,[17][32][33][34][35]such as social and cognitive reinforcement of incorrect memories[36][37]or false news reports and misleading photographs that influence the formation of memories based on them.[26][38][37][39]

Theories[edit]

Fuzzy-trace theory[edit]

The fuzzy-trace theory, proposed by Valerie Reyna and Charles Brainerd, suggests that information can be stored in two different ways: verbatim and gist.[40]Verbatim representations are literal, precise, and exact copies of the information. On the other hand, gist representations are fuzzy, general, and abstracted representations of the information.

The fuzzy-trace theory relates to false memory because studies have found that when information is stored as a gist representation, it is more prone to manipulation.[41]This is because information is stored in parallel pathways, not together. The term verbatim traces applies to the storing of surface features of an experience, whereas gist traces applies to the way in which we store the context and interpretation of an event.[42]

Strength hypothesis[edit]

The strength hypothesis states that instrong situations(situations where one course of action is encouraged more than any other course of action due to the objective payoff), people are expected to demonstrate rational behavior, basing their behavior on the objective payoff.[43]

An example of this is the laws of a country. Most of its citizens, no matter how daring, will conform to these laws, because the objective payoff is personal safety.

Construction hypothesis[edit]

The construction hypothesis says that if a true piece of information being provided can alter a respondent's answer, then so can a false piece of information.[44]

Construction hypothesis has major implications for explanations on the malleability of memory. Upon asking a respondent a question that provides a presupposition, the respondent will provide a recall in accordance with the presupposition (if accepted to exist in the first place). The respondent will recall the object or detail.[44]

Skeleton theory[edit]

Loftus developed what some refer to as "the skeleton theory" after having run an experiment involving 150 subjects from theUniversity of Washington.[44]Loftus noticed that when a presupposition was one of false information it could only be explained by the construction hypothesis and not the strength hypothesis. Loftus then stated that a theory needed to be created for complex visual experiences where the construction hypothesis plays a significantly more important role than situational strength. She presented a diagram as a "skeleton" of this theory, which later became referred to by some as the skeleton theory.

The skeleton theory explains the procedure of how a memory is recalled, which is split into two categories: the acquisition processes and the retrieval processes.

The acquisition processes are in three separate steps. First, upon the original encounter, the observer selects a stimulus to focus on. The information that the observer can focus on compared to all of the information occurring in the situation as a whole, is very limited. In other words, a lot is going on around us and we only pick up on a small portion. This forces the observer to begin by selecting a focal point for focus. Second, our visual perception must be translated into statements and descriptions. The statements represent a collection of concepts and objects; they are the link between the event occurrence and the recall. Third, the perceptions are subject to any "external" information being provided before or after the interpretation. This subsequent set of information can reconstruct the memory.[44]

The retrieval processes come in two steps. First, the memory and imagery are regenerated. This perception is subject to what foci the observer has selected, along with the information provided before or after the observation. Second, the linking is initiated by a statement response, "painting a picture" to make sense of what was observed. This retrieval process results in either an accurate memory or a false memory.[44]

Natural factors for the formation of false memories[edit]

Individual differences[edit]

Greater creative imagination and dissociation are known to relate to false memory formation.[45]Creative imagination may lead to vivid details of imagined events. High dissociation may be associated with habitual use of lax response criteria for source decisions due to frequent interruption of attention or consciousness. Social desirability and false memory have also been examined.[46]Social desirability effects may depend on the level of perceived social pressure.[45]

Individuals who feel under greater social pressure may be more likely to acquiesce. Perceived pressure from an authority figure may lower individuals' criteria for accepting a false event as true. The new individual difference factors include preexisting beliefs about memory, self-evaluation of one's own memory abilities, trauma symptoms, andattachmentstyles. Regarding the first of these, metamemory beliefs about the malleability of memory, the nature oftrauma memory,and the recoverability of lost memory may influence willingness to accept vague impressions or fragmentary images as recovered memories and thus, might affect the likelihood of accepting false memory.[47]For example, if someone believes that memory once encoded is permanent, and that visualization is an effective way to recover memories, the individual may endorse more liberal criteria for accepting a mental image as true memory. Also, individuals who report themselves as having better everyday memories may feel more compelled to come up with a memory when asked to do so. This may lead to more liberal criteria, making these individuals more susceptible to false memory.

There is some research that shows individual differences in false memory susceptibility are not always large (even on variables that have previously shown differences—such as creative imagination or dissociation[48]), that there appears to be no false memory trait,[49][50]and that even those who have highly superior memory are susceptible to false memories.[51]

Trauma[edit]

A history of trauma is relevant to the issue of false memory. It has been proposed that people with a trauma history or trauma symptoms may be particularly vulnerable to memory deficits, including source-monitoring failures.[52]

Possible associations between attachment styles and reports of false childhood memories were also of interest. Adult attachment styles have been related to memories of early childhood events, suggesting that the encoding or retrieval of such memories may activate the attachment system. It is more difficult for avoidant adults to access negative emotional experiences from childhood, whereas ambivalent adults access these kinds of experiences easily.[53]Consistent with attachment theory, adults with avoidant attachment styles, like their child counterparts, may attempt to suppress physiological and emotional reactions to activation of the attachment system. Significant associations between parental attachment and children'ssuggestibilityexist. These data, however, do not directly address the issue of whether adults' or their parents' attachment styles are related to false childhood memories. Such data nevertheless suggest that greater attachment avoidance may be associated with a stronger tendency to form false memories of childhood.

Sleep deprivation[edit]

Sleep deprivation can also affect the possibility of falsely encoding a memory. In two experiments, participants studied DRM lists (lists of words [e.g., bed, rest, awake, tired] that are semantically associated with a non-presented word) before a night of either sleep or sleep deprivation; testing took place the following day. One study showed higher rates of false recognition in sleep-deprived participants, compared with rested participants.[54]

Sleep deprivation can increase the risk of developing false memories. Specifically, sleep deprivation increased false memories in a misinformation task when participants in a study were sleep deprived during event encoding, but did not have a significant effect when the deprivation occurred after event encoding.[55]

False memory syndrome[edit]

False memory syndromeis defined as false memory being a prevalent part of one's life in which it affects the person's mentality and day-to-day life. False memory syndrome differs from false memory in that the syndrome is heavily influential in the orientation of a person's life, while false memory can occur without this significant effect. The syndrome takes effect because the person believes the influential memory to be true.[56]However, its research is controversial and the syndrome is not included in theDiagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders.False memory is an important part of psychological research because of the ties it has to a large number of mental disorders, such asPTSD.[57]False memory can be declared a syndrome when recall of a false or inaccurate memory takes great effect on a person's life. Such a false memory can completely alter the orientation of one's personality and lifestyle.[1][58]

Psychotherapy[edit]

Therapists who subscribe to recovered memory theory point to a wide variety of common problems, ranging from eating disorders to sleeplessness, as evidence ofrepressed memoriesof sexual abuse.[59]Psychotherapists tried to reveal "repressed memories" in mental therapy patients through "hypnosis, guided imagery, dream interpretation and narco-analysis". The reasoning was that if abuse could not be remembered, then it needed to be recovered by the therapist. The legal phenomena developed in the 1980s, with civil suits alleging child sexual abuse on the basis of "memories" recovered during psychotherapy. The term "repressed memory therapy" gained momentum and with it social stigma surrounded those accused of abuse. The "therapy" led to other psychological disorders in persons whose memories were recovered.[medical citation needed]

Memories recovered through therapy have become more difficult to distinguish between simply beingrepressedor having existed in the first place.[60][61]

Therapists have used strategies such ashypnotherapy,repeated questioning, andbibliotherapy.These strategies may provoke the recovery of nonexistent events or inaccurate memories.[62][63][64]A recent report indicates that similar strategies may have produced false memories in several therapies in the century before the modern controversy on the topic which took place in the 1980s and 1990s.[65]

According to Loftus, there are different possibilities to create false therapy-induced memory. One is the unintentional suggestions of therapists. For example, a therapist might tell their client that, on the basis of their symptoms, it is quite likely that they had been abused as a child. Once this "diagnosis" is made, the therapist sometimes urges the patient to pursue the recalcitrant memories. It is a problem resulting from the fact that people create their own social reality with external information.[66]

The "lost-in-the-mall" techniqueis another recovery strategy.[clarification needed]This is essentially a repeated suggestion pattern. The person whose memory is to be "recovered" is persistently said to have gone through an experience even if it may have not happened. This strategy can cause the person to recall the event as having occurred, despite its falsehood.[67]

Hypnosis[edit]

Laurence and Perry conducted a study testing the ability to induce memory recall through hypnosis. Subjects were put into a hypnotic state and later woken up. Observers suggested that the subjects were woken up by a loud noise. Nearly half of the subjects being tested concluded that this was true, despite it being false. However, by therapeutically altering the subject's state, they may have been led to believe that what they were being told was true.[68]Because of this, the respondent has a false recall.

A 1989 study focusing on hypnotizability and false memory separated accurate and inaccurate memories recalled. In open-ended question formation, 11.5% of subjects recalled the false event suggested by observers. In a multiple-choice format, no participants claimed the false event had happened. This result led to the conclusion that hypnotic suggestions produce shifts in focus, awareness, and attention. Despite this, subjects do not mix fantasy up with reality.[1]

Effects on society[edit]

Legal cases[edit]

Therapy-induced memory recovery has made frequent appearances in legal cases, particularly those regarding sexual abuse.[69]Therapists can often aid in creating a false memory in a victim's mind, intentionally or unintentionally. They may associate a patient's behavior with the fact that they have been a victim of sexual abuse, thus helping the memory occur. They may then use memory enhancement techniques such as hypnosis dream analysis in the attempt to extract memories of sexual abuse from victims. InRamona v. Isabella,two therapists wrongly prompted a recall that their patient, Holly Ramona, had been sexually abused by her father. It was suggested that the therapist, Isabella, had implanted one of the sexual abuse memories in Ramona after use of the hypnotic drugsodium amytal.After a nearly unanimous decision, Isabella had been declarednegligenttowards Holly Ramona. This 1994 legal issue played a massive role in shedding light on the possibility of false memories' occurrences.[70]

In another legal case where false memories were used, they helped a man to be acquitted of his charges. Joseph Pacely had been accused of breaking into a woman's home with the intent to sexually assault her. The woman had given her description of the assailant to police shortly after the crime had happened. During the trial, memory researcher Elizabeth Loftus testified that memory is fallible and there were many emotions that played a part in the woman's description given to police. Loftus has published many studies consistent with her testimony.[44][71][72]These studies suggest that memories can easily be changed around and sometimes eyewitness testimonies are not as reliable as many believe.

Another notable case is Maxine Berry. Berry grew up in the custody of her mother, who opposed the father having contact with her. When the father expressed his desire to attend his daughter's high school graduation, the mother enrolled Berry in therapy, ostensibly to deal with the stress of seeing her father. The therapist pressed Berry to recover memories of sex abuse by her father. Berry broke down under the pressure and had to be psychiatrically hospitalized. She underwenttubal ligation,so she would not have children and repeat the cycle of abuse. With the support of her husband and primary care physician, Berry eventually realized that her memories were false and filed a suit for malpractice. The suit brought to light the mother's manipulation of mental health professionals to convince Berry that she had been sexually abused by her father. In February 1997, Berry sued her therapists[73]and clinic that treated her from 1992 to 1995 and, she says, made her falsely believe she had been sexually and physically abused as a child when no such abuse ever occurred. The lawsuit, filed in February 1997 in Minnehaha Co. Circuit Court South Dakota, states that therapist Lynda O'Connor-Davis had an improper relationship with Berry, both during and after her treatment. The suit also names psychologist Vail Williams, psychiatrist Dr. William Fuller and Charter Hospital and Charter Counseling Center as defendants. Berry and her husband settled out of court.[74]

Although there have been many legal cases in which false memory appears to have been a factor, this does not ease the process of distinguishing between false memory and real recall. Sound therapeutic strategy can help this differentiation, by either avoiding known controversial strategies or to disclosing controversy to a subject.[62][1][75]

Harold Merskey published a paper on the ethical issues of recovered-memory therapy.[75]He suggests that if a patient had pre-existing severe issues in their life, it is likely that "deterioration" will occur to a relatively severe extent upon memory recall. This deterioration is a physical parallel to the emotional trauma being surfaced. There may be tears, writhing, or many other forms of physical disturbance. The occurrence of physical deterioration in memory recall coming from a patient with relatively minor issues prior to therapy could be an indication of the recalled memory's potential falsehood.[75]

Children[edit]

False memory is often considered for trauma victims[76]including those ofchildhood sexual abuse.[77][78][62][79]

If a child experienced abuse, it is not typical for them to disclose the details of the event when confronted in an open-ended manner.[80]Trying to indirectly prompt a memory recall can lead to the conflict of source attribution, as if repeatedly questioned the child might try to recall a memory to satisfy a question. The stress being put on the child can make recovering an accurate memory more difficult.[77]Some people hypothesize that as the child continuously attempts to remember a memory, they are building a larger file of sources that the memory could be derived from, potentially including sources other than genuine memories. Children that have never been abused but undergo similar response-eliciting techniques can disclose events that never occurred.[80]

One of children's most notable setbacks in memory recall issource misattribution.Source misattribution is the flaw in deciphering between potential origins of a memory. The source could come from an actual occurring perception, or it can come from an induced and imagined event. Younger children, preschoolers in particular, find it more difficult to discriminate between the two.[81]Lindsay & Johnson (1987) concluded that even children approachingadolescencestruggle with this, as well as recalling an existent memory as a witness. Children are significantly more likely to confuse a source between being invented or existent.[82]

Shyamalan, Lamb and Sheldrick (1995) partially re-created a study that involved attemptedmemory implantingin children. The study comprised a series of interviews concerning a medical procedure that the children may have undergone. The data was scored so that if a child made one false affirmation during the interview, the child was classified as inaccurate. When the medical procedure was described in detail, "only 13% of the children answered 'yes' to the question 'Did you ever have this procedure?'". As to the success of implantation with false 'memories', the children "assented to the question for a variety of reasons, a false memory being only one of them. In sum, it is possible that no false memories have been created in children inimplanted-memorystudies ".[83]

Ethics and public opinion[edit]

A 2016 study surveyed the public's attitude regarding the ethics of planting false memories as an attempt to influence healthy behavior. People were most concerned with the consequences, with 37% saying it was overly manipulative, potentially harmful or traumatic. Their reasons against are that the ends do not justify the means (32%), potential for abuse (14%), lack of consent (10%), practical doubts (8%), better alternative (7%), and free will (3%). Of those who thought implanting false memories would be acceptable, 36% believed the end justified the means, with other reasons being increasing treatment options (6%), people need support (6%), no harm would be done (6%), and it's no worse than alternatives (5%).[84]

Potential benefits[edit]

Several possible benefits associated with false memory arrive fromfuzzy-trace theoryand gist memory. Valerie F. Reyna, who coined the terms as an explanation for the DRM paradigm, explains that her findings indicate that reliance on prior knowledge from gist memory can help individuals make safer, well informed choices in terms of risk taking.[85]Other positive traits associated with false memory indicate that individuals have superior organizational processes, heightened creativity, and prime solutions for insight based problems. All of these things indicate that false memories are adaptive and functional.[86]False memories tied to familiar concepts can also potentially aid in future problem solving in a related topic, especially when related to survival.[87]

See also[edit]

  • Source-monitoring error,an effect in which memories are incorrectly attributed to different experiences from the ones that caused them.
  • Déjà vu,the feeling that one has lived through the present situation before.
  • Jamais vu,the feeling of unfamiliarity with recognized memories.
  • Cryptomnesia,a memory that is not recognized as such.
  • Lost in the mall technique,a memory implantation technique used to demonstrate that false memories can be implanted through suggestions made to experimental subjects in which the subject is told that an older relative was present at the time.
  • Memory conformity
  • Inception,a science fiction film dealing with the concept of implanting ideas in sleeping individuals.

References[edit]

  1. ^abcdGleaves, David H.; Smith, Steven M.; Butler, Lisa D.; Spiegel, David (2006). "False and Recovered Memories in the Laboratory and Clinic: A Review of Experimental and Clinical Evidence".Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice.11:3–28.doi:10.1093/clipsy.bph055.
  2. ^Knafo, D. (2009). Freud's memory erased.Psychoanalytic Psychology, 26(2), 171–190.doi:10.1037/a0015557
  3. ^Zongwill, O. L. (2019). Memory abnormality. Retrieved fromhttps:// britannica /science/memory-abnormalityArchived30 June 2020 at theWayback Machine
  4. ^abcLoftus, Elizabeth F.; Palmer, John C. (1974). "Reconstruction of automobile destruction: An example of the interaction between language and memory".Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior.13(5): 585–589.doi:10.1016/s0022-5371(74)80011-3.
  5. ^Goldschmied, Nadav; Sheptock, Mark; Kim, Kacey; Galily, Yair (2017). "Appraising Loftus and Palmer (1974) Post-Event Information versus Concurrent Commentary in the Context of Sport".Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology.70(11): 2347–2356.doi:10.1080/17470218.2016.1237980.PMID27643483.S2CID205924157.
  6. ^Beaver, David I. (1 April 2011)."Presupposition".Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy.Archivedfrom the original on 1 May 2020.Retrieved13 May2020.
  7. ^Loftus, Elizabeth, F. (1975). "Reconstructing memory: The incredible eyewitness".Jurimetrics Journal.15(3): 188–193.{{cite journal}}:CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  8. ^Malpass, Roy S.; Goodman-Delahunty, Jane (2004). "Psychology and the Law, Overview".Encyclopedia of Applied Psychology.pp. 171–184.doi:10.1016/B0-12-657410-3/00380-9.ISBN978-0-12-657410-4.
  9. ^abLampinen, James M.; Neuschatz, Jeffrey S.; Payne, David G. (1997). "Memory illusions and consciousness: Examining the phenomenology of true and false memories".Current Psychology.16(3–4): 181–224.doi:10.1007/s12144-997-1000-5.S2CID144603817.
  10. ^McDermott, K. B., & Roediger III, H. L. (1998). Attempting to avoid illusory memories: Robust false recognition of associates persists under conditions of explicit warnings and immediate testing.Journal of Memory and Language,39(3), 508-520.
  11. ^Ihlebæk, C., Løve, T., Erik Eilertsen, D., & Magnussen, S. (2003). Memory for a staged criminal event witnessed live and on video.Memory,11(3), 319-327.
  12. ^Storbeck, J.; Clore, G.L. (2005). "With Sadness Comes Accuracy; with Happiness, False Memory: Mood and the False Memory Effect".Psychological Science.16(10): 785–791.doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01615.x.PMID16181441.S2CID16608445.
  13. ^Bhengu, Cebelihle (6 November 2019)."Hundreds 'remember' Nelson Mandela dying in the 1980s: Inside the Mandela Effect".TimesLIVE.Retrieved5 June2024.
  14. ^"Two cognitive psychologists explain the mystery of the 'Mandela effect'".The Independent.Archivedfrom the original on 12 November 2020.Retrieved3 October2018.
  15. ^de Vito, Stefania; Cubelli, Roberto; Della Sala, Sergio (May 2009). "Collective representations elicit widespread individual false memories".Cortex.45(5): 686–687.doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2008.08.002.PMID19111288.
  16. ^Emery, David (24 July 2016)."The Mandela Effect".Snopes.Archivedfrom the original on 17 February 2022.Retrieved21 September2017.
  17. ^ab"Are you living in an alternate reality? Welcome to the wacky world of the 'Mandela Effect'".The Telegraph.London. 20 September 2016.Archivedfrom the original on 12 January 2022.Retrieved21 September2017.
  18. ^Tweed, Carter (29 May 2017)."Did the Fruit of the Loom logo have a cornucopia?".Alternate Memories.Archivedfrom the original on 30 January 2021.Retrieved25 November2020.
  19. ^Palma, Bethania (20 July 2022)."No, Darth Vader Didn't Actually Say 'Luke, I Am Your Father'".Snopes.Retrieved6 December2022.
  20. ^Prasad, Deepasri; Bainbridge, Wilma A. (2022)."The Visual Mandela Effect as Evidence for Shared and Specific False Memories Across People".Psychological Science.33(12): 1971–1988.doi:10.1177/09567976221108944.ISSN1467-9280.PMID36219739.S2CID241793849.
  21. ^"The Mandela Effect: Poll shows most Americans misremember Darth Vader quote, Monopoly logo".ABC7 Los Angeles.5 September 2022.Retrieved6 December2022.
  22. ^Bannon, Jessica Lyn (16 January 2022)."Rich Uncle Pennybags didn't wear a monocle — and other false memories".The Hill.Retrieved6 December2022.
  23. ^abTait, Amelia (21 December 2016)."The movie that doesn't exist and the Redditors who think it does".New Statesman.London.Archivedfrom the original on 26 March 2021.Retrieved21 September2017.
  24. ^28 December 1994, on the cable channelTNT;the marathon featured movies includingSinbad and the Eye of the Tiger(1977).
  25. ^Evon, Dan (28 December 2016)."Did Sinbad Play a Genie in the 1990s Movie 'Shazaam'?".Snopes.Archivedfrom the original on 19 December 2021.Retrieved12 February2020.
  26. ^abAamodt, Caitlin (16 February 2016)."Collective False Memories: What's Behind the 'Mandela Effect'?".Discover Magazine.Archivedfrom the original on 25 November 2020.Retrieved12 December2019.
  27. ^Hankins, Sarah (21 March 2017)."If You Remember 'Shazaam', The Movie That Doesn't Exist, You Aren't Alone".The Odyssey Online.Archivedfrom the original on 2 February 2021.Retrieved12 February2020.
  28. ^Drinkwater, Ken; Dagnall, Neil (19 July 2019)."The 'Mandela effect' and the science of false memories".The Conversation.Retrieved4 March2022.
  29. ^"Critically Thinking About the Mandela Effect | Psychology Today".psychologytoday.Retrieved4 March2022.
  30. ^Dimou, Eleni (30 December 2021)."The Science Behind the Reality-Bending Mandela Effect".Popular Mechanics.Retrieved4 March2022.
  31. ^Beran, Dale (30 July 2019).It Came from Something Awful: How a Toxic Troll Army Accidentally Memed Donald Trump into Office(1st ed.). New York:All Points Books.p. 16.ISBN978-1-250-21947-3.
  32. ^"Collective False Memories: What's Behind the 'Mandela Effect'?".The Crux.16 February 2017. Archived fromthe originalon 28 February 2017.Retrieved27 February2017.
  33. ^Fernando, Gavin (8 November 2016)."Does this picture look a bit off to you?".News.au.Archivedfrom the original on 11 November 2020.Retrieved27 February2017.
  34. ^Cooke, Henry (12 October 2017)."NZ and the 'Mandela Effect': Meet the folks who remember New Zealand being in a different place".Stuff.Archivedfrom the original on 10 January 2021.Retrieved27 February2017.
  35. ^Gabbert, Elisa(9 February 2017)."On a Grandma's House and the Unknowability of the Past".Pacific Standard.Archivedfrom the original on 17 February 2022.Retrieved1 March2017.
  36. ^Brown, Adam D.; Kouri, Nicole; Hirst, William (23 July 2012)."Memory's Malleability: Its Role in Shaping Collective Memory and Social Identity".Frontiers in Psychology.3:257.doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00257.ISSN1664-1078.PMC3402138.PMID22837750.
  37. ^ab"Can groups of people" remember "something that didn't happen?".Hopes&Fears.Archivedfrom the original on 18 January 2021.Retrieved1 March2017.
  38. ^"False Memories in the News: Are Pictures Worth MORE Than 1,000 Words?".CogBlog – A Cognitive Psychology Blog.Archivedfrom the original on 25 February 2021.Retrieved1 March2017.
  39. ^Resnick, Brian (20 April 2018)."We're underestimating the mind-warping potential of fake video".Vox.Archivedfrom the original on 15 March 2021.Retrieved20 December2018.
  40. ^Miller, Patricia; Bjorklund, David (1998). "Contemplating Fuzzy-Trace Theory: The Gist of it".Journal of experimental child psychology.71(2) 184-193
  41. ^Bialer, Daniel; Reyna, Valerie; Brainerd, Charles (2021). "False Memory: What are the Effects, How Does Fuzzy-Trace Theory Predict Them, and How Does This Matter For Eyewitness Testimony?".Methods, measures, and theories in eyewitness identification tasks.325-352
  42. ^Brainerd, C.J.; Reyna, V.F. (October 2002)."Fuzzy-Trace Theory and False Memory".Current Directions in Psychological Science.11(5): 164–169.doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00192.ISSN0963-7214.
  43. ^Lozano, J. H. (2018). The situational strength hypothesis and the measurement of personality.Social Psychological and Personality Science, 9(1), 70-79.doi:10.1177/1948550617699256
  44. ^abcdefLoftus, Elizabeth F. (1975). "Leading questions and the eyewitness report".Cognitive Psychology.7(4): 560–572.doi:10.1016/0010-0285(75)90023-7.S2CID16731808.
  45. ^abOst, James; Foster, Samantha; Costall, Alan; Bull, Ray (2005)."False reports of childhood events in appropriate interviews".Memory.13(7): 700–710.doi:10.1080/09658210444000340.PMID16191820.S2CID33621927.Archivedfrom the original on 27 March 2020.Retrieved27 March2020.
  46. ^Paddock, John R.; Terranova, Sophia; Kwok, Rosie; Halpern, David V. (2000). "When Knowing Becomes Remembering: Individual Differences in Susceptibility to Suggestion".The Journal of Genetic Psychology.161(4): 453–468.doi:10.1080/00221320009596724.PMID11117101.S2CID34798568.
  47. ^Ghetti, Simona; Bunge, Silvia A. (2012)."Neural changes underlying the development of episodic memory during middle childhood".Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.2(4): 381–395.doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2012.05.002.PMC3545705.PMID22770728.
  48. ^Patihis, L.; Frenda, S.; Loftus, E.F. (2018)."False memory tasks do not reliably predict other false memories".Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice.5(2): 140–160.doi:10.1037/cns0000147.S2CID150202452.Archivedfrom the original on 23 October 2020.Retrieved27 May2020.
  49. ^Bernstein, D.M.; Scoboria, A.; Desjarlais, L.; Soucie, K. (2018). ""False memory" is a linguistic convenience ".Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice.5(2): 161–179.doi:10.1037/cns0000148.S2CID173992031.
  50. ^Patihis, L. (2018)."Why there is no false memory trait and why everyone is susceptible to memory distortions: The dual encoding interference hypothesis".Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice.5(2): 180–184.doi:10.1037/cns0000143.S2CID149974111.Archivedfrom the original on 22 October 2020.Retrieved27 May2020.
  51. ^Patihis, L.; Frenda, S.J.; LePort, A.K.R.; Petersen, N.; Nichols, R.M.; Stark, C.E.L.; McGaugh, J.L.; Loftus, E.F. (2013)."False memories in highly superior autobiographical memory individuals".PNAS.110(52): 20947–20952.Bibcode:2013PNAS..11020947P.doi:10.1073/pnas.1314373110.PMC3876244.PMID24248358.
  52. ^Bremner, J. Douglas; Shobe, Katharine Krause; Kihlstrom, John F. (2000). "False Memories in Women with Self-Reported Childhood Sexual Abuse: An Empirical Study".Psychological Science.11(4): 333–337.doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00266.PMID11273395.S2CID6113518.
  53. ^Edelstein, Robin S. (2006). "Attachment and emotional memory: Investigating the source and extent of avoidant memory impairments".Emotion.6(2): 340–345.CiteSeerX10.1.1.498.181.doi:10.1037/1528-3542.6.2.340.PMID16768567.
  54. ^Diekelmann, Susanne; Landolt, Hans-Peter; Lahl, Olaf; Born, Jan; Wagner, Ullrich (23 October 2008)."Sleep Loss Produces False Memories".PLOS ONE.3(10): e3512.Bibcode:2008PLoSO...3.3512D.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0003512.ISSN1932-6203.PMC2567433.PMID18946511.
  55. ^Frenda, Steven J.; Patihis, Lawrence; Loftus, Elizabeth F.; Lewis, Holly C.; Fenn, Kimberly M. (2014)."Sleep Deprivation and False Memories".Psychological Science.25(9): 1674–1681.doi:10.1177/0956797614534694.PMID25031301.S2CID339864.Archivedfrom the original on 18 October 2019.Retrieved1 December2018.
  56. ^Kaplan, Robert; Manicavasagar, Vijaya (2001). "Is there a false memory syndrome? A review of three cases".Comprehensive Psychiatry.42(4): 342–348.doi:10.1053/comp.2001.24588.PMID11936143.
  57. ^Friedman, Matthew J. (1996)."PTSD diagnosis and treatment for mental health clinicians".Community Mental Health Journal.32(2): 173–189.doi:10.1007/bf02249755.PMID8777873.S2CID143532335.
  58. ^Kihlstrom, J.F.(1998).Exhumed memoryArchived1 October 2021 at theWayback Machine.In S.J. Lynn & K.M. McConkey (Eds.),Truth in memory,(pp. 3-31). New York: Guilford.
  59. ^Mertz, Elizabeth; Bowman, Cynthia (1998),The Clinical Corner: Third-Party Liability in Repressed Memory Cases: Recent Legal Developments,doi:10.1037/e300392004-003
  60. ^Otgaar, Henry; Howe, Mark L.; Patihis, Lawrence; Merckelbach, Harald; Lynn, Steven Jay; Lilienfeld, Scott O.; Loftus, Elizabeth F. (November 2019)."The Return of the Repressed: The Persistent and Problematic Claims of Long-Forgotten Trauma".Perspectives on Psychological Science.14(6): 1072–1095.doi:10.1177/1745691619862306.hdl:11343/245210.PMC6826861.PMID31584864.
  61. ^Watters, Ethan (27 September 2022)."Opinion | The Forgotten Lessons of the Recovered Memory Movement".The New York Times.Retrieved16 January2024.
  62. ^abcWare, Robert C. (1995). "Scylla and Charybdis".Journal of Analytical Psychology.40(1): 5–22.doi:10.1111/j.1465-5922.1995.00005.x.PMID7868381.
  63. ^McElroy, Susan L.;Keck, Paul E. (1995). "Recovered Memory Therapy: False Memory Syndrome and Other Complications".Psychiatric Annals.25(12): 731–735.doi:10.3928/0048-5713-19951201-09.
  64. ^Gold, Steven N. (1997). "False memory syndrome: A false dichotomy between science and practice".American Psychologist.52(9): 988–989.doi:10.1037/0003-066x.52.9.988.
  65. ^Patihis, Lawrence; Younes Burton, Helena J. (2015)."False memories in therapy and hypnosis before 1980".Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice.2(2): 153–169.doi:10.1037/cns0000044.
  66. ^Loftus, Elizabeth F. (1993). "The reality of repressed memories".American Psychologist.48(5): 518–537.doi:10.1037/0003-066x.48.5.518.PMID8507050.
  67. ^Loftus, E.F. (2005)."Planting misinformation in the human mind: A 30-year investigation of the malleability of memory".Learning & Memory.12(4): 361–366.doi:10.1101/lm.94705.PMID16027179.
  68. ^Laurence, Jean-Roch; Perry, Campbell (1983). "Hypnotically Created Memory among Highly Hypnotizable Subjects".Science.222(4623): 523–524.Bibcode:1983Sci...222..523L.doi:10.1126/science.6623094.PMID6623094.
  69. ^DePrince, Anne P.; Brown, Laura S.; Cheit, Ross E.; Freyd, Jennifer J.; Gold, Steven N.; Pezdek, Kathy; Quina, Kathryn (18 October 2011), "Motivated Forgetting and Misremembering: Perspectives from Betrayal Trauma Theory",True and False Recovered Memories,vol. 58, New York: Springer, pp. 193–242,doi:10.1007/978-1-4614-1195-6_7,ISBN978-1-4614-1194-9,PMID22303768
  70. ^"Spectral Evidence - The Ramona Case: Incest, Memory, and Truth on Trial in Napa Valley".Women's Rights Law Reporter.20:49. 1998–1999.Archivedfrom the original on 20 November 2021.Retrieved17 February2022.
  71. ^Powers, Peter A.; Andriks, Joyce L.; Loftus, Elizabeth F. (1979). "Eyewitness accounts of females and males".Journal of Applied Psychology.64(3): 339–347.doi:10.1037/0021-9010.64.3.339.S2CID31305222.
  72. ^Loftus, Elizabeth F.; Greene, Edith (1980). "Warning: Even memory for faces may be contagious".Law and Human Behavior.4(4): 323–334.doi:10.1007/BF01040624.S2CID146947540.
  73. ^"FMSF Newsletter Archive".False Memory Syndrome Foundation. 1 April 1997.Archivedfrom the original on 9 October 2019.Retrieved26 October2018.
  74. ^Gerrietts, Jennifer (27 February 1997)."Hospital's staff falsely made woman believe she was abused, lawsuit says".Argus-Leader.p. 27.Archivedfrom the original on 22 October 2020.Retrieved27 April2020.
  75. ^abcMerskey, Harold (1996)."Ethical Issues in the Search for Repressed Memories".American Journal of Psychotherapy.50(3): 323–335.doi:10.1176/appi.psychotherapy.1996.50.3.323.PMID8886232.
  76. ^Carey, Benedict; Hoffman, Jan (25 September 2018)."They Say Sexual Assault, Kavanaugh Says It Never Happened: Sifting Truth From Memory".The New York Times.Archivedfrom the original on 26 September 2018.Retrieved26 September2018.Memory, by its nature and necessity, is selective, its details subject to revision and dissipation.... "Recollection is always a reconstruction, to some extent — it's not a videotape that preserves every detail," said Richard J. McNally, a professor of psychology at Harvard University and the author ofRemembering Trauma."The details are often filled in later, or dismissed, and guessing may become part of the memory."...Recalling an event draws on some of same areas of the brain that recorded it; in essence, to remember is to relive. Every time the mind summons the encoded experience, it can add details, subtract others and even alter the tone and point of the story. That reassembly, in turn, is freshly stored again, so that the next time it comes to mind it contains those edits. Using memory changes memory, as cognitive scientists say. For a victim, often the only stable elements are emotions and the tunnel-vision details: the dress she wore, the hand over her mouth
  77. ^abBremner, J.D.; Krystal, J.H.; Charney, D.S.; Southwick, S.M. (1996). "Neural Mechanisms in dissociative amnesia for childhood abuse: Relevance to the current controversy surrounding the" false memory syndrome "".American Journal of Psychiatry.153(7): 71–82.doi:10.1176/ajp.153.7.71.PMID8659644.
  78. ^Davis, Joseph E. (2005). "Victim Narratives and Victim Selves: False Memory Syndrome and the Power of Accounts".Social Problems.52(4): 529–548.doi:10.1525/sp.2005.52.4.529.
  79. ^Christianson, Sven-åke; Loftus, Elizabeth F. (1987). "Memory for traumatic events".Applied Cognitive Psychology.1(4): 225–239.doi:10.1002/acp.2350010402.
  80. ^abCeci, Stephen J.; Loftus, Elizabeth F.; Leichtman, Michelle D.; Bruck, Maggie (1994). "The Possible Role of Source Misattributions in the Creation of False Beliefs Among Preschoolers".International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis.42(4): 304–320.doi:10.1080/00207149408409361.PMID7960288.
  81. ^Foley, Mary Ann; Johnson, Marcia K. (1985). "Confusions between Memories for Performed and Imagined Actions: A Developmental Comparison".Child Development.56(5): 1145–55.doi:10.2307/1130229.JSTOR1130229.PMID4053736.
  82. ^Lindsay, D. Stephen; Johnson, Marcia K.; Kwon, Paul (1991). "Developmental changes in memory source monitoring".Journal of Experimental Child Psychology.52(3): 297–318.doi:10.1016/0022-0965(91)90065-z.PMID1770330.
  83. ^Goodman, Gail S; Quas, Jodi D; Redlich, Allison A (1998)."The ethics of conducting 'false memory' research with children: a reply to Herrmann and Yoder"(PDF).Applied Cognitive Psychology.12(3): 207–217.doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199806)12:3<207::AID-ACP523>3.0.CO;2-T.Archived fromthe original(PDF)on 9 August 2017.Retrieved10 July2019.
  84. ^Nash, Robert A.; Berkowitz, Shari R.; Roche, Simon (2016)."Public Attitudes on the Ethics of Deceptively Planting False Memories to Motivate Healthy Behavior".Applied Cognitive Psychology.30(6): 885–897.doi:10.1002/acp.3274.PMC5215583.PMID28111495.
  85. ^Dodgson, Lindsay.Our brains sometimes create 'false memories' — but science suggests we could be better off this wayArchived6 June 2020 at theWayback MachineBusiness Insider.
  86. ^“Future Thinking and False Memories.” Psychology Today, Sussex Publishers, psychologytoday /us/blog/time-travelling-apollo/201607/future-thinking-and-false-memories.
  87. ^Jarrett, Christian (23 September 2013)."False memories have an upside".British Psychological Society.Archivedfrom the original on 21 November 2018.Retrieved21 November2018.

Further reading[edit]

Listen to this article(30minutes)
Spoken Wikipedia icon
This audio filewas created from a revision of this article dated 30 July 2019(2019-07-30),and does not reflect subsequent edits.