Graphical perception
Graphical perceptionis the human capacity forvisuallyinterpreting information ongraphs and charts.Both quantitative and qualitative information can be said to be encoded into the image, and the human capacity to interpret it is sometimes called decoding.[1]The importance of human graphical perception, what we discern easily versus what our brains have more difficulty decoding, is fundamental to goodstatistical graphicsdesign, where clarity, transparency, accuracy and precision in data display and interpretation are essential for understanding the translation of data in a graph to clarify and interpret the science.[2][3][4][5][6][7]
Graphical perception is achieved in dimensions or steps of discernment by:
- detection:recognition of geometry which encodes physical values
- assembly:grouping of detected symbol elements; discerning overall patterns in data
- estimation:assessment of relative magnitudes of two physical values.
Cleveland and McGill's experiments[1]to elucidate the graphical elements humansdetectmost accurately is a fundamental component of goodstatistical graphicsdesign principles.[2][3][5][6][8][9][10][11][12]In practical terms, graphs displaying relative position on a common scale most accurately are most effective. A graph type that utilizes this element is thedot plot.Conversely, angles are perceived with less accuracy; an example is thepie chart.Humans do not naturally order color hues. Only a limited number of hues can be discriminated in one graphic.
Graphic designs that utilize visualpre-attentive processingin the graph design'sassemblyis why a picture can be worth a thousand words by using the brain's ability to perceive patterns. Not all graphs are designed to consider pre-attentive processing. For example in the attached figure, a graphic design feature, table look-up, requires the brain to work harder and take longer to decode than if the graph utilizes our ability to discern patterns.[3]
Graphic design that readily answers the scientific questions of interest will include appropriateestimation.Details for choosing the appropriate graph type for continuous andcategorical dataand for grouping have been described.[6][13]Graphics principles for accuracy, clarity and transparency have been detailed[2][3][4][14]and key elements summarized.[15]
See also
[edit]References
[edit]- ^abCleveland, William; McGill, Robert (1984). "Graphical Perception and Graphical Methods for Analyzing Scientific Data".Journal of the American Statistical Association.79(387): 531–544.doi:10.1080/01621459.1984.10478080.JSTOR2288400.
- ^abcCleveland, William (1993).Visualizing Data.Summit, New Jersey: Hobart Press.ISBN0-9634884-0-6.
- ^abcdCleveland, William (1994).The elements of graphing data.Summit, New Jersey: Hobart Press.ISBN0-9634884-1-4.
- ^abTufte, Edward (2001).The Visual Display of Quantitative Information.Cheshire, Connecticut: Graphics Press.ISBN1930824130.
- ^abHarrell, Jr, Frank (April 24, 2017)."PRINCIPLES OF GRAPH CONSTRUCTION"(PDF).Vanderbilt.RetrievedJune 9,2018.
- ^abcDuke, Susan; Bancken, Fabrice; Crowe, Brenda; Soup, Mat; Botsis, Taxiarchis; Forshee, Richard (2015). "Seeing is believing: good graphic design principles for medical research".Statistics in Medicine.34(22): 3040–3059.doi:10.1002/sim.6549.PMID26112209.S2CID36793148.
- ^Angra, Aakanksha; Gardner, Stephanie (2017)."Reflecting on Graphs: Attributes of Graph Choice and Construction Practices in Biology".CBE: Life Sciences Education.16(3): ar53.doi:10.1187/cbe.16-08-0245.PMC5589433.PMID28821538.
- ^Cleveland, William; McGill, Robert (1985). "Graphical Perception and Graphical Methods for Analyzing Scientific Data".Science.229(4716): 828–833.Bibcode:1985Sci...229..828C.doi:10.1126/science.229.4716.828.PMID17777913.S2CID16342041.
- ^Robbins, Naomi (2005).Creating More Effective Graphs.Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. pp. 47–62.ISBN0985911123.
- ^Carswell, C. Melody (1992). "Choosing Specifiers: An Evaluation of the Basic Tasks Model of Graphical Perception".Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society.34(5): 535–554.doi:10.1177/001872089203400503.PMID1459565.S2CID19818462.
- ^Hollands, J. G.; Spence, Ian (1992). "Judgments of Change and Proportion in Graphical Perception".Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society.34(3): 313–334.doi:10.1177/001872089203400306.PMID1634243.S2CID28097022.
- ^"Graph Design Rule #2: Explain your encodings".Flowing Data.2010-08-26.RetrievedJune 9,2018.
- ^Bancken, Fabrice (September 6, 2012)."Select the Right Graph".CTSpedia Safety Graphics Home.RetrievedJune 10,2018.
- ^Harrell, Jr, Frank (April 24, 2017)."Graphics for Clinical Trials".Vanderbilt Dept of Biostatistics.RetrievedJune 10,2018.
- ^Lane, Peter; Duke, Susan (Aug 12, 2012)."Best Practices Recommendations".CTSpedia Safety Graphics Home.RetrievedJune 10,2018.
External links
[edit]- Abrief description and picture of Cleveland and McGill's nine graphical elements
- "How William Cleveland Turned Data Visualization Into a Science"(2016) from Priceonomics
- John Rauser's 2016 presentation, "How Humans See Data"at Velocity Amsterdam. Describes how good visualizations optimize for the human visual system
- Michael Friendly'sGallery of Data Visualization: The Best and Worst of Statistical Graphics